Vision Standards for Aircrew:
Visual Acuity for Pilots
Completed by:
Jason K. Kumagai, Sheri Williams & Donald Kline*
5 Corvus Court,
Ottawa ON K2E 7Z4
*Professor & Director, Vision & Aging Lab
Department of Psychology, PACE Program
Department of Surgery, Division of Ophthalmology
University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive N.W.
Calgary, AB T2N 1N4
PWGSC Contract No. W7711-047921/001/TOR
On behalf of
DEPARTMENT OF NATIONAL DEFENCE
as represented by
Defence Research and Development Canada
1133 Sheppard Avenue West
P.O. Box 2100
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
DRDC - Toronto Scientific Authority
Sharon McFadden
DRDC Toronto CR 2005-142
March 2005
This page intentionally left blank
The scientific or technical validity of this Contract Report is entirely the responsibility of the contractor and the
contents do not necessarily have the approval or endorsement of the Defence R&D Canada.
UNCLASSIFIED
Vision Standards for Aircrew:
Visual Acuity for Pilots
Completed by:
Kumagai, J.K., Williams, S.L. & Kline, D.W.*
Greenley and Associates Incorporated
5 Corvus Court,
Ottawa ON K2E 7Z4
www.greenley.ca
*Professor & Director, Vision & Aging Lab
Department of Psychology, PACE Program
Department of Surgery, Division of Ophthalmology
University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive N.W.
Calgary, AB T2N 1N4
Project Manager: Jason K. Kumagai
PWGSC Contract No. W7711-047921/001/TOR
On behalf of
DEPARTMENT OF NATIONAL DEFENCE
as represented by
Defence Research and Development Canada
1133 Sheppard Avenue West
P.O. Box 2100
Toronto, Ontario
DRDC - Toronto Scientific Authority
Sharon McFadden
416 635-2189
DRDC Toronto CR 2005-142
March 2005
The scientific or technical validity of this Contract Report is entirely the responsibility of the contractor and the
contents do not necessarily have the approval or endorsement of the Defence R&D Canada.
UNCLASSIFIED
Sponsored by:
Defence Research and Development Canada Toronto
1133 Sheppard Avenue West
P.O. Box 2000
Toronto Ontario
M3M 3B9
DRDC – Toronto Scientific Authority
Sharon McFadden
Copyright:
© HER MAJESTY THE QUEEN IN RIGHT OF CANADA (2005)
AS REPRESENTED BY THE Minister of National Defence
© SA MAJESTE LA REINE EN DROIT DUE CANADA (2005)
Defence National Canada
Vision Standards for Aircrew
i
Abstract
This report documents a study investigating the Canadian Forces (CF) aircrew entrance
vision standard. A literature review was conducted to identify a method for establishing
bona fide occupational requirements and validated standards for aircrew related visual
functions. A protocol for establishing and validating an occupationally based visual
acuity standard for the CF pilot occupation was selected. Tasks that have critical visual
acuity functions were identified based on data obtained through questionnaires and a
focus group session. The study proposes potential task simulations that accurately reflect
critical aircrew tasks and an experimental plan to establish vision standards.
Résumé
Ce rapport présente une étude portant sur la norme visuelle fixée pour le personnel
navigant au niveau d’entrée en fonction des Forces canadiennes. Une revue de la
littérature visant à trouver une méthode pour établir des exigences professionnelles
justifiées et des normes validées pour les fonctions visuelles du personnel navigant a été
menée. Un protocole d’établissement et de validation d’une norme d’acuité visuelle pour
la profession de pilote des FC a été choisi. Les tâches comportant des fonctions
essentielles liées à l’acuité visuelle ont été définies à partir de données tirées de
questionnaires et d’une séance de discussion en groupe. L’étude propose des simulations
de tâches qui reflètent avec exactitude les tâches essentielles du personnel navigant et un
plan expérimental de fixation des normes visuelles.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
ii
This page intentionally left blank
Vision Standards for Aircrew
iii
Executive Summary
The Canadian Forces (CF) requires justification for the current visual acuity recruitment
standard for the CF aircrew community. Information was derived from a systematic
review of literature focused on the visual functions important in flight operations, a task
questionnaire sent to pilots, and a pilot Subject Matter Expert (SME) discussion. A
review of available literature revealed no studies or other evaluations that supported or
explained the origin of any of the present CF vision standards. No literature was
available to substantiate the borrowing of vision standards from other occupations. For
these reasons, it is recommended that vision standard development for CF pilots must
take into account their own specialized tasks and the environments in which they work.
An experimental plan is proposed for assessing the near and far visual acuity
requirements of pilots.
The proposed experimental plans involve the conduct of two simulations designed to
obtain objective evidence of the relationship between visual acuity level and pilot task
performance. The experiments will simulate two piloting tasks identified as representing
the highest level of visual acuity demand for pilots. These piloting tasks were identified
as requiring excellent visual acuity across all aircraft types. One simulation tests near
acuity, with participants reading approach plates during an approach/landing at night.
The second simulation tests far visual acuity, with participants locating and identifying
air and ground traffic/obstacles during an approach/landing task. Experiments will use
positive sphere lenses to examine the effect of parametrically degraded acuity on task
performance. It is proposed that the participants will be tested at Best, 6/9, 6/12 and 6/18
in the near acuity simulation and Best, 6/9, 6/12, 6/18 and 6/24 in the far acuity
simulation. The results of these simulations will be used to determine the minimum
visual acuity requirement for CF pilots.
It is proposed that both experiments should be conducted in a flight simulator. This will
allow standardization of lighting levels, environmental conditions and control of size and
distance of test items. Investigation into realistic environmental conditions will be
conducted to determine if experimentation can be conducted in field conditions.
However, consideration must be given to the tradeoff between conducting task
simulations in realistic environments and having complete control over the experimental
environment.
Consideration is given to the appropriateness of using only visual acuity as the sole
measure to test vision. Other components of visual function, their associated diagnostic
tests, and levels appropriate to flight operations were investigated. Visual functions
included contrast sensitivity, visual fields, glare sensitivity, colour vision, night vision,
depth perception, and motion perception. While there are many potential measures of
visual function, there is no evidence at this time to suggest that any visual function is
more valid than visual acuity for assessing pilot task performance.
Consideration is also given to optical correction. CF policy is to recruit candidates based
upon an uncorrected visual acuity standard, yet many intermediate and senior level pilots
currently rely upon vision correction. Refractive errors necessitating the use of corrective
lenses are increasingly prevalent with increasing age. The likelihood of age-related
changes in refractive error, an older recruiting age, and the probable requirement for
Vision Standards for Aircrew
iv
visual correction with age, highlight a need to consider the recognition of a corrected
visual acuity standard. Several studies have shown that corrective lenses can be used
safely and effectively in aviation. However, there is some evidence that corrected lenses
contribute to a higher incidence of accidents, reduce identification capability in combat
maneuvers, and contribute to aviation mishaps.
Aspects of refractive error and photorefractive surgery were investigated because
consideration of candidates with corrective eye surgery into the CF pilot occupation
would have a bearing on the visual acuity standard. Currently, personnel with corrective
eye surgery are not eligible for entry into the CF pilot occupation. Concerns include the
structural stability of the eye as well as the effects on visual functioning post-surgery.
In order to perform the simulation experiments proposed in this report, a number of
action items are recommended. These include (but are not limited to) the following
items: determine a suitable simulation test bed or field trial for performing the
experiments; measurement of stimulus dimension and environmental variables likely to
be encountered operationally; determine the feasibility and timeframe associated with
developing the simulator software and hardware changes that will be required in order to
conduct the experiments; verify the tasks and associated visual metrics with SMEs;
develop an experimental protocol for evaluating the simulation tests and obtain Human
Ethics Committee approval; determine a suitable population for participating in the
experiments based upon age, experience, gender, aircraft type flown, etc; and conduct,
administer and validate a visual function test for a sample of aircrew in order to ascertain
their suitability for participating in the experiments. Addressing and resolving these
items will be the focus of the next phase of the project.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
v
Sommaire
Les Forces canadiennes (FC) ont besoin d’une justification à l’appui de la norme d’acuité
visuelle actuellement en vigueur pour le recrutement du personnel navigant des FC.
L’information provient d’une revue systématique de la littérature traitant des fonctions
visuelles importantes dans les opérations aériennes, d’un questionnaire sur les tâches
envoyé aux pilotes et d’une discussion avec les experts en la matière. L’examen de la
littérature a révélé qu’aucune étude ou évaluation ne soutenait ni n’expliquait l’origine
des normes visuelles en vigueur actuellement dans les FC. Aucun ouvrage ne justifiait
l’emprunt de normes visuelles à d’autres professions. Il est donc recommandé que
l’élaboration d’une norme visuelle pour les pilotes des FC tienne compte des tâches
spécialisées et de l’environnement de travail qui leur sont propres. Un plan expérimental
est proposé pour évaluer les exigences liées à l’acuité visuelle de près et de loin des
pilotes.
Les plans expérimentaux proposés prévoient deux simulations visant à recueillir des
preuves objectives de la relation entre le niveau d’acuité visuelle et l’exécution des tâches
de pilotage. Ces plans simuleront deux tâches de pilotage représentant celles qui exigent
le niveau maximal d’acuité visuelle chez les pilotes. Il a été établi que ces tâches exigent
une excellente acuité visuelle pour tous les types d’aéronef. La première simulation, qui
consiste à demander aux participants de lire les instructions au cours d’une approche/un
atterrissage de nuit, évalue l’acuité visuelle de près. La deuxième, au cours de laquelle les
participants repèrent la circulation/les obstacles aériens et les obstacles au sol pendant
une approche/un atterrissage, évalue l’acuité visuelle de loin. À l’aide de lentilles
sphériques positives, on examinera l’effet de la dégradation paramétrique de l’acuité sur
l’exécution des tâches. Il est proposé que les participants soient évalués selon la
correction optimale, 6/9, 6/12 et 6/18 dans la simulation portant sur l’acuité de près selon
la correction optimale, 6/9, 6/12, 6/18 et 6/24 dans la simulation portant sur l’acuité de
loin. Les résultats de ces simulations serviront à déterminer l’exigence minimale en
matière d’acuité visuelle pour les pilotes des FC.
Il est proposé que les deux expériences se tiennent dans un simulateur de vol afin que les
niveaux d’éclairage, les conditions environnementales et le contrôle de la taille et de la
distance des items de test puissent être normalisés. Des recherches seront menées dans
des conditions environnementales réalistes afin de déterminer si l’expérimentation peut se
faire dans des conditions naturelles. Il importe toutefois d’étudier l’arbitrage entre la
tenue des simulations de tâche dans des environnements réalistes et le contrôle total sur
l’environnement expérimental.
L’étude se penche sur l’opportunité d’évaluer la vision en se fondant uniquement sur
l’acuité visuelle. D’autres éléments de la fonction visuelle, les tests diagnostiques
associés et les niveaux adaptés aux opérations aériennes ont été étudiés. Les fonctions
visuelles comprennent la sensibilité au contraste, les champs visuels, la sensibilité à
l’éblouissement, la vision des couleurs, la vision nocturne, la perception de la profondeur
et la perception du mouvement. Il y a plusieurs mesures possibles de la fonction visuelle,
mais rien ne prouve à l’heure actuelle qu’aucune fonction visuelle ne permet mieux de
mesurer la capacité d’exécuter les tâches de pilotage que l’acuité visuelle.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
vi
L’étude se penche aussi sur la correction optique. Selon la politique des FC, le
recrutement des candidats se fonde sur une norme d’acuité visuelle non corrigée, mais
bon nombre de pilotes de niveau intermédiaire et supérieur ont actuellement une
correction de la vue. La fréquence des vices de réfraction exigeant le port de lentilles
correctrices augmente avec l’âge. La nécessité d’envisager l’adoption d’une norme
d’acuité visuelle corrigée se fait sentir étant donné que des changements dans la
fréquence des vices de réfraction et de la correction visuelle en raison de l’âge sont à
prévoir et que l’âge du recrutement est repoussé. Plusieurs études ont montré que des
lentilles correctrices peuvent être utilisées efficacement et sans danger dans le domaine
de l’aviation. Certaines données indiquent toutefois que les lentilles correctrices
entraînent un taux plus élevé d’accidents, réduisent la capacité d’identification dans les
manœuvres de combat et jouent un rôle dans les catastrophes aériennes.
Certains aspects des vices de réfraction et de la chirurgie photoréfractive ont été étudiés
puisque la prise en considération, de candidats ayant subi une chirurgie de correction de
la vue pour un poste de pilote dans les FC, aurait une incidence sur la norme d’acuité
visuelle. Le personnel qui a subi une chirurgie de correction de la vue ne peut
actuellement exercer la profession de pilote dans les FC. Les inquiétudes ont trait à la
stabilité structurale de l’œil et aux effets de la chirurgie sur le fonctionnement visuel.
Un certain nombre de mesures sont recommandées pour que l’on puisse mener les
expériences de simulation proposées dans ce rapport. Ce sont notamment les suivantes :
choisir un banc d’essai ou un essai sur le terrain pour la tenue des expériences; mesurer la
dimension des stimuli et les variables environnementales susceptibles d’être présentes
dans les opérations; déterminer s’il est possible d’apporter au logiciel et au matériel du
simulateur les changements nécessaires à la tenue des expériences et le délai à prévoir
pour y arriver; vérifier les tâches et les mesures visuelles connexes avec les experts en la
matière; mettre au point un protocole expérimental pour l’évaluation des tests de
simulation et obtenir l’approbation du Comité de déontologie humaine; déterminer une
population qui pourrait prendre part aux expériences en se fondant notamment sur l’âge,
l’expérience, le sexe et le type d’aéronef utilisé; et tenir, administrer et valider un test de
fonction visuelle pour un échantillon du personnel navigant afin de vérifier s’il peut
prendre part aux expériences. L’étude et le règlement de ces points seront au cœur de la
prochaine étape du projet.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
vii
Table of Contents
Abstract................................................................................................................................ i
Executive Summary...........................................................................................................iii
Table of Contents..............................................................................................................vii
List of Figures.................................................................................................................... ix
Acknowledgements............................................................................................................. x
1 Introduction................................................................................................................. 1
1.1 Background......................................................................................................... 1
1.1.1 Canadian Human Rights Act ...................................................................... 2
1.1.2 Determining a Vision Standard................................................................... 3
1.2 Current Vision Standard ..................................................................................... 3
1.2.1 Report Outline............................................................................................. 4
2 Methodology............................................................................................................... 5
2.1 Literature Review................................................................................................ 5
2.2 Task Analysis...................................................................................................... 6
2.2.1 Pilot Vision Task Questionnaire................................................................. 6
2.2.2 Subject Matter Expert Focus Group Session.............................................. 7
2.3 Propose Task Simulations and Experimental Design......................................... 9
3 Results - Visual Functions........................................................................................ 10
3.1 Far Acuity ......................................................................................................... 10
3.1.1 Description................................................................................................ 10
3.1.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 12
3.1.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 13
3.2 Near Acuity....................................................................................................... 14
3.2.1 Description................................................................................................ 14
3.2.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 15
3.2.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 18
3.3 Contrast Sensitivity (CS) .................................................................................. 18
3.3.1 Description................................................................................................ 18
3.3.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 20
3.3.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 21
3.4 Visual Fields and Useful Field of View............................................................ 22
3.4.1 Visual Fields ............................................................................................. 22
3.4.2 Useful Field of View................................................................................. 24
3.5 Glare Sensitivity & Recovery........................................................................... 25
3.5.1 Disability Glare......................................................................................... 25
3.5.2 Discomfort Glare ...................................................................................... 26
3.5.3 Glare Recovery ......................................................................................... 27
3.6 Colour Vision.................................................................................................... 28
3.6.1 Description................................................................................................ 28
3.6.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 29
3.6.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 30
3.7 Night Vision...................................................................................................... 30
3.7.1 Description................................................................................................ 30
3.7.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 31
3.7.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 32
Vision Standards for Aircrew
viii
3.8 Depth Perception............................................................................................... 33
3.8.1 Description................................................................................................ 33
3.8.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 33
3.8.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 34
3.9 Motion Perception............................................................................................. 34
3.9.1 Description................................................................................................ 34
3.9.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 35
3.9.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 36
3.10 Refractive Error and Optical Correction........................................................... 37
3.10.1 Description................................................................................................ 37
3.10.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 38
3.10.3 Related Tasks............................................................................................ 39
3.11 Refractive Error and Photorefractive Surgery .................................................. 39
3.11.1 Description................................................................................................ 39
3.11.2 Literature Review...................................................................................... 40
4 Results - Tasks.......................................................................................................... 41
4.1 Common Tasks Performed by CF Pilots .......................................................... 41
4.1.1 Vision Task Questionnaire Results........................................................... 42
4.1.2 SME Discussion........................................................................................ 43
4.2 Consequences of Improper Performance.......................................................... 44
4.3 Task Simulations............................................................................................... 44
4.3.1 Near Visual Acuity Task........................................................................... 44
4.3.2 Far Visual Acuity Task............................................................................. 45
5 Results - Proposed Test Scenarios............................................................................ 47
5.1 Lab-Based Simulation....................................................................................... 48
5.2 Near Visual Acuity Test Scenario..................................................................... 48
5.2.1 Participants................................................................................................ 49
5.2.2 Method...................................................................................................... 49
5.2.3 Results....................................................................................................... 49
5.3 Far Visual Acuity Test Scenario....................................................................... 49
5.3.1 Participants................................................................................................ 49
5.3.2 Method...................................................................................................... 50
5.3.3 Results....................................................................................................... 50
5.4 General Experimental Plan............................................................................... 50
5.4.1 Clarification of Tasks................................................................................ 50
5.4.2 Randomizing Conditions .......................................................................... 51
6 Discussion................................................................................................................. 52
6.1 Visual Acuity .................................................................................................... 52
6.1.1 Near Acuity Vision Standard.................................................................... 52
6.1.2 Far Acuity Vision Standard ...................................................................... 52
6.2 Potential Visual Functions to Consider in a Vision Standard........................... 53
6.3 Adoption of a Corrected Visual Acuity Standard............................................. 54
7 Conclusion and Recommendations........................................................................... 55
8 Appendix A - Vision Task Questionnaire................................................................. 56
9 Appendix B - Acronyms........................................................................................... 63
10 Appendix C - References.......................................................................................... 64
Vision Standards for Aircrew
ix
List of Figures
Figure 3.1. Examples of high-contrast acuity targets (Optotypes). .................................. 10
Figure 3.2. The effects of age-related accommodative loss on focus and eye-strain with
extended viewing for observers with low-accommodation. Source: Kline, Caird, Ho,
& Dewar (2002)........................................................................................................ 15
Figure 3.3. Approach plate for Greater Moncton International Airport – Atlantic Region.
Source: Geomatics Canada, Dept of Natural Resources, 2005................................. 17
Figure 3.4. A sample Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Pelli, D. G., Robson, J. G.,
& Wilkins, A. J., 1988. Copyright © 2002 D.G. Pelli and J.G. Robson. Distributed
by Haag-Streit........................................................................................................... 19
Figure 3.5. Sample VCTS 6500 contrast sensitivity chart. Source: Vistech Consultants
(1988)........................................................................................................................ 20
Vision Standards for Aircrew
x
Acknowledgements
The experimenters would like to express their sincere appreciation to each of the
participants (the subject matter experts), for their time and effort in support of the project.
The participants were all very knowledgeable, helpful and cooperative. Also, Sharon
McFadden and Commander Cyd E. Courchesne have been instrumental in providing
scientific advice and locating information sources. A special thanks to Major Larry F.
Green and Captain Frank B. Cannon for their correspondence and coordination efforts.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
1
1 Introduction
This report documents a study investigating the Canadian Forces aircrew visual acuity
entrance standard. The objectives of this project were to:
1. Review the currently available literature on the establishment of bona fide
occupational requirements and validated standards for vision;
2. Identify a suitable protocol for establishing and validating an occupationally
based vision standard for aircrew;
3. Select (or design) simulation tests that accurately reflect critical tasks of aircrew;
and,
4. Recommend a visual acuity standard and an appropriate test procedure for aircrew
selection.
This work was sponsored by Defence Research and Development Canada (DRDC) -
Toronto and was completed by Greenley & Associates Incorporated (G&A) under
PWGSC Contract No. W7711-047921/001/TOR.
1.1 Background
Changes in human rights legislation and court challenges have helped to prompt
increased efforts to develop and validate selection standards that are occupationally and
medically relevant. It is now being recognized that a listing of essential functions
without a linkage to occupational requirements is incomplete at best. Vision selection
standards are critical to the effective and safe conduct of many tasks and this is
particularly evident for many of the tasks involved in flight operations. Good vision is a
vital requirement for mission success in many aviation tasks. Although the importance of
other sensory systems in completing complex tasks should not be overlooked, vision is
the only sensory system that is likely to be used to its fullest capacity during flying tasks
(Swamy, Joseph, Aravind & Veval, 2002).
The level of visual functioning necessary to effectively conduct essential job functions is
the only appropriate basis for a professionally appropriate vision standard. Efforts to
develop relevant vision selection standards were initiated in a study, contracted by the
Surgeon General, to investigate the feasibility of developing valid vision standards for all
Canadian Forces (CF) occupations. Further study was contracted by the Directorate of
Health Services Delivery to develop a suitable test methodology for establishing
standards for land, sea, air and support environments (Casson, 1995). Air Command
tasked DRDC Toronto to review the CF aircrew vision standards and propose
amendments that considered operational conditions currently encountered by aircrew.
An aircrew survey was conducted that identified tasks perceived to be visually
demanding and gathered informed opinions on potential vision standards (Heikens, Gray,
O’Neill & Salisbury, 1999). However, the survey did not validate the suggested vision
standards, nor did it link existing and recommended vision standards to task performance.
The work conducted in the current study builds upon previous efforts to establish a valid,
occupationally and medically relevant standard for CF pilot vision.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
2
To ensure that the entrance standard for pilots is set at a level that is both fair and safe,
the CF required a review of the current visual acuity standard for the pilot community.
Specifically, the “uncorrected visual acuity standard” (i.e. the medical standard for the
ability to see detail without the use of spectacle or contact lens correction) is to be
determined. This requires determining the level of visual acuity required to perform pilot
tasks safely, efficiently and to an acceptable performance level.
This document outlines the methodology, results and proposed simulations to be
administered in order to develop a bona fide visual acuity standard for CF pilots. This
included an extensive literature review used to develop a work plan to establish and
validate a vision standard for aircrew. Critical vision tasks were identified that can be
used in experimentation to propose a vision standard directly associated with task
performance. Simulation tests that accurately reflect the critical tasks with high visual
acuity demand were developed for testing purposes to ensure that the acuity standard is
both occupationally and medically relevant, as well as compliant with human rights
legislation.
The importance of a bona fide vision standard will help to ensure that qualified
candidates are not excluded. The rejection of qualified persons imposes great costs to the
CF considering all of the recruitment, testing, training and selection costs are lost.
Further to this, a qualified individual may be rejected for erroneous reasons. Moving
down the hiring list for selection also imposes risk due to the selection of a lesser
qualified candidate, as the placement on a hiring list is reflective of potential job
performance (Carmean, 1998).
The scope of this project work was to support the requirement of DRDC Toronto to
develop, validate and implement a task-oriented, performance-based visual acuity
standard for Canadian Forces pilot recruitment purposes.
1.1.1 Canadian Human Rights Act
The Canadian Human Rights Act prohibits the denial of employment opportunity (i.e.,
the training, initiation or continuation of employment) to an individual on the basis of
disability unless it can be demonstrated that the applicant cannot meet the Bona Fide
Occupational Requirements (BFOR) of the job. According to the Act, whenever an
employer devises methods of testing an individual's performance of a job, the procedure
must include:
1. Identification of the essential tasks which make up the requirements of the job;
2. Identification of the skills and capabilities required to perform the essential tasks
of the job;
3. Methods which evaluate the ability of the individual to carry out the essential
tasks of the job by any reasonable method; and
4. Standards which do not exceed the minimum requirements of the job.
Canadian Human Rights Reporter Supplement, 1982 TR/82-3
Vision Standards for Aircrew
3
Any study designed to determine appropriate and defensible vision standards must meet
the requirements of this legislation in order to be justifiable and fair and must be
supportable with evidence rather than opinion alone.
In addition to demonstrating that certain levels of visual acuity are required for effective
and safe performance, it is also necessary to show that there is a real and substantial risk
to people and property if this standard is not met.
1.1.2 Determining a Vision Standard
A fair and effective vision standard must be based on the Bona Fide Occupational
Requirements (BFOR) for visual acuity. This requires an analysis of the tasks conducted
by pilots and an identification of those tasks that:
1. Are essential to program completion;
2. Present a high risk of property damage and/or personal injury if performed
incorrectly; and
3. Have a high visual acuity requirement.
These tasks will represent the BFOR for a visual acuity standard. They will also form the
basis of an experimental design that will help to determine the visual acuity standard
necessary to perform these tasks in an operational environment.
1.2 Current Vision Standard
Currently, the Canadian Forces Pilot recruitment standard is based upon a number of
physical tests, including multiple vision tests. These vision tests include the following
items:
Distance visual acuity: 6/6 in better eye and 6/9 in the other eye (both
uncorrected)
Near visual acuity: N5 (reading distance), N14 (100cm) in the better eye and N6
(reading distance), N18 (100cm) in the worse eye
Refractive error (in diopters): +2.50 Hyperopia, -0.25 Myopia, + 0.75
Astigmatism and allowable Cyclopegia
Heterophoria (in prism diopters): No more than 10 Exophoria, 10 Esophoria and 2
Hyperphoria
Visual fields: clinical confrontation test and, if indicated, Humphries VF test
Stereoacuity: Titmus Tester
Interocular pressure: 22mmHg or less with the Goldman applanation tonometry
Colour vision: If <17/21 Pseudoisochromatic colour plates (PIP) test + Blue-
Yellow plate for tritan defect, then tested with Holmes-Wright lantern, or
Farnsworth Lantern assessment. If one or more errors occur on either lantern test,
then the applicant is determined to be CV3
Detection of previous Keratorefractive surgery: use the standard for Corneal
topography as indicated in INFO PUB 61/115/22
Vision Standards for Aircrew
4
The current study is focused on the CF pilot visual acuity recruitment standard, although
consideration has been provided for other vision parameters and possible simulation of
these parameters (see Section 6 Discussion).
1.2.1 Report Outline
This report documents the following items:
Introduction - provides the background information and report outline;
Method - outlines the information gathering framework and methodology;
Results -
o Visual Functions – defines various visual parameters, associated literature
and commentary from focus group discussions.
o Visual Correction – discussion of optical correction and photorefractive
surgery.
o Tasks – outline of essential tasks that have a critical vision component and
recommendations for tasks to simulate.
o Proposed Test Scenarios – an experimental plan to assess the visual acuity
recruitment standard of CF pilots.
Discussion - the vision-related functions and variables that should be considered;
and
Conclusion and Recommendations – action items required in developing a bona
fide vision standard.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
5
2 Methodology
The methodology used to gather information to develop a defensible, task-oriented visual
acuity standard for the CF pilot occupation, is outlined below. This methodology is
based on previous experience and an extensive review of literature on visual functions in
flying. The methodology includes a review of literature and legal challenges and the
identification of tasks that have critical visual acuity functions, based on data obtained
through questionnaires and a focus group session. The results of these tasks were
analyzed to propose task scenarios that accurately reflect critical aircrew tasks and an
experimental plan to establish vision standards.
2.1 Literature Review
In order to determine if the current visual acuity standard is reasonable, a comprehensive
review of scientific literature from both military and civilian sources was completed. A
literature review of the existing Canadian Forces visual acuity pilot standards for
recruiting was conducted, as well as a review of the visual acuity standards in other
countries. A brief search was also performed regarding recent relevant legal challenges
and the Canadian Human Rights Act to determine the requirements for a ‘Bona Fide
Occupational Vision Requirement’. Further, a brief internet search was performed to
gather information related to visual acuity and high demand tasks. Scientific and medical
literature related to pilot tasks and visual acuity was reviewed and the task analysis also
included a review of the Occupational Analysis Unit of the Directorate of Manpower
Planning literature. Literature was reviewed on visual functions and task performance in
related occupations in order to assess the existing evidence on task based visual acuity
requirements. A number of vision standards developed for other occupations (e.g.,
police, divers) and non-military aircrew also provided guidance to the project objectives.
EndNote, an electronic bibliographic database, was used to collect and organize reference
materials.
The literature review provided the following list of parameters of visual functions which
should be considered by the CF:
1. Monocular and binocular uncorrected visual acuity (UCVA): UCVA refers to
visual acuity tested without optical correction. What determines the minimum
requirement for acuity without correction (i.e. what determines safe performance
in an emergency when spectacles are lost)?
2. Monocular and binocular best corrected visual acuity (BCVA): BCVA refers to
acuity tested with the best-possible optical correction in place. What determines
the minimum requirement for acuity with correction?
3. Degraded Visual Environments: How are the visual abilities listed above
influenced by poor environmental conditions?
4. Pilot Aging: How do age-related changes in vision affect performance on pilot
tasks as a function of viewing conditions?
Vision Standards for Aircrew
6
These parameters were considered throughout the task analyses as well as the test
scenarios that were developed to study the effect of degraded vision on the performance
of CF pilot tasks.
A review of LegalTrac articles was conducted for legal challenges related to vision
standards for pilots. No articles were identified that related to the Canadian Forces and
vision standards for pilots. One case was identified involving piloting tasks. An Arizona
jury awarded substantial damages to a promising young commercial pilot grounded for
life after Lasik surgery, due to poor preoperative screening. Although he still has at least
20/20 vision, side effects from the surgery impaired his ability to see clearly at night. He
saw glare from landing lights. (Holt, 2002)
Findings of the literature review are integrated into the results below.
2.2 Task Analysis
The emphasis on a task-oriented and performance-based vision standard was a major
component of the project objectives. Only by assessing the vision standard against
critical user tasks will the CF be able to ensure it is applicable and valid. A bona fide
occupational vision standard must be based on demonstrations that the standard is
actually required to perform job-related tasks. Using non-task-related vision
requirements has ramifications that are more detrimental than an occasional lawsuit, as it
can result in the placement of persons in positions in which they cannot perform the
essential functions of their job.
The task analysis was conducted in two main thrusts; a preliminary questionnaire analysis
and a focus group session with experienced pilots representing all aircraft types.
2.2.1 Pilot Vision Task Questionnaire
The literature was reviewed to identify particular tasks associated with high degrees of
risk in terms of pilot safety. A questionnaire was generated based on tasks identified in
the Vision Survey of CF Aircrew document (Heikens et al., 1999). This Aircrew
Operational Vision Survey was sent to all operating CF pilots. Pilots were asked to
answer a number of questions related to the visual demands of the tasks they perform.
The results of the survey were analyzed in terms of the type of aircraft flown. Aircraft
were divided into the following groups:
Tactical Helicopter
Search and Rescue Rotary Wing
Maritime Patrol Rotary Wing
Transport
Maritime Patrol Fixed Wing
Fighter
Primary Rotary Wing Trainer
Primary Fixed Wing Trainer
Vision Standards for Aircrew
7
The results of the survey identified the highest rated visually demanding tasks as
indicated by the CF pilots, for each aircraft type.
Using the results of the 1997 Vision Survey of CF Aircrew (Heikens et al., 1999), a
questionnaire was developed for the current project to assist in identifying the critical,
high risk tasks associated with military piloting occupations. The tasks that received a
high rating in terms of visual demand from the survey were selected for the Pilot Vision
Study Questionnaire (Appendix A) which was sent to subject matter experts (SMEs) prior
to the focus group session. Each pilot was asked to fill in the applicable survey section
based upon the type of aircraft they were currently flying. The pilots were asked the
following questions in relation to the visually demanding tasks they perform:
What type of task is this? (Routine/ Non-Routine/ Emergency)
What is the vision requirement for performing this task? (Near vision/
Intermediate vision/ Far vision/ Variable vision requirements)
What kind of environmental conditions is the task performed under? (Bright
sunshine/ Rain/ Fog/ Snow/ Bright sunshine + Ground snow/ Night/ Dusk or
Dawn)
What is the lowest level of experience required to perform this task? (New recruit
(0-2 years of experience)/ Junior pilot (2-5 years of experience)/ Intermediate
pilot (5-10 years of experience)/ Senior pilot (10 + years of experience)
The results from this questionnaire allowed the experimenters to focus on specific tasks
that were analyzed in greater detail with the pilots in an SME session. Tasks that were
selected for additional analysis were mission and safety critical tasks, tasks common
across all aircraft, emergency tasks and difficult tasks to perform in terms of visual
acuity.
This process identified specific tasks to analyze in more detail with the SMEs in terms of
demand for visual acuity, which was subsequently required for the selection of
experimental test scenarios.
2.2.2 Subject Matter Expert Focus Group Session
A Subject Matter Expert (SME) focus group session was conducted to further investigate
the visual acuity requirements for CF pilots. The objective of the focus group was to
assess the current CF pilot visual acuity entrance standard, verify the tasks performed by
CF pilots, discuss the visual demands of these tasks and develop an outline for a task-
based simulation providing justification and guidance to update the entrance standard.
Twelve SMEs participated in a focus group discussion. All SMEs had flying experience
with a variety of aircraft and came from different Canadian Airforce bases (12 Wing
Shearwater, Central Flying School Wing, Transport Rescue Standardization/ Evaluation
Team Trenton, 1 Wing Headquarters Kingston and MPSET Greenwood). The
participants’ average total flight time was 6220 hours.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
8
The SME session provided information to determine the essential tasks performed by CF
pilots that have a critical vision component. The SME session was designed and
conducted to:
Discuss tasks performed by CF pilots
Validate the task list across all aircraft
Determine the tasks common across all aircraft
Discuss the pilot tasks in terms of mission criticality and safety
Determine and characterize the visual characteristics of critical pilot tasks
Review emergency procedures and critical incidents
Discuss comprehensive and realistic pilot flying scenarios,
Breakdown the scenarios into component tasks and
Determine the tasks that may be feasible to simulate
This process was used to ensure that the final standard meets the requirements of the
Canadian Human Rights Act.
The SME session was led with a dual perspective approach, focusing first on tasks and
secondly on visual functions. The task portion of the SME session was designed to
gather information related to vision critical tasks based on previous research, such as the
vision survey. The discussion also identified tasks that are common across all aircraft
types including training and operational conditions. The second portion of the SME
session focused on the visual functions and characterized elements of pilot tasks that
made them visually demanding.
2.2.2.1 Critical Incident Documentation (Behavioural Examples)
Critical incidents were discussed during the SME session. This process involved
obtaining information about previous incidents related to task performance. The SMEs
were asked to recall particular incidents of either outstanding or inferior job performance,
or situations where a particular incident occurred or a specific ability was required. For
each behavioural example the information obtained included:
1. The circumstances that preceded the incident;
2. What the employee did specifically that was effective or ineffective, or required a
specific ability; and
3. The consequences or result of the incident in question.
The SMEs were asked to provide particular behavioural examples where vision was
involved in these critical incidents.
A summary of the task analysis results, including the results from the Vision Tasks
Questionnaire and SME session is provided in the results section.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
9
2.3 Propose Task Simulations and Experimental Design
Upon completion of the SME Sessions, an experimental plan was developed. Two test
scenarios involving simulation experiments were developed to test the effects of varied
visual acuity on the task performance of CF pilots. The first experiment is a near visual
acuity task that requires participants to extract critical task information from approach
plates during an approach/landing at night. The second experiment is a distance visual
acuity task that involves locating and identifying ground traffic/obstacles during an
approach (landing) or reconnaissance. The goal of both simulations is to provide a
controlled experimental environment that simulates the visual acuity aspects of each task
and allows for objective measurements of performance. A proposed experimental design
is outlined in the Results section of this report.
It should be noted that information related to pilot tasks and procedures in this report has
been generated directly from the SME session participants and is therefore, based on
personal descriptions of their tasks and procedures. As a result, the tasks and procedures
described in this report do not necessarily correspond directly with published regulations
or procedures.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
10
3 Results - Visual Functions
The results of the study are presented in this section and the following three sections of
the report. The visual functions in aviation, including optical correction and
photorefractive surgery are discussed in Section 3, SME questionnaire results in Section
4, the tasks selected for simulation in Section 5 and the proposed experimental plan to
simulate these tasks in Section 6.
Although there is no doubt that military flying is visually demanding (e.g., Sekuler, Kline
& Dismukes, 1982), existing knowledge does not allow precise specification of the visual
functions that are critical to aircrew performance (e.g., Casson, 1995). Two reasons
account for this: first, relatively few experimental studies directly relate to this issue, and
secondly, the data that are available are generally not sufficient to allow quantitative
characterization of specific visual functions for complex dynamic tasks such as flying an
aircraft. Thus, the literature review that follows will explore the relationship between
different aspects of vision and aircrew tasks as well as those in related task domains.
3.1 Far Acuity
3.1.1 Description
Resolution acuity refers to the smallest detail (gap or feature) that can be resolved,
usually in a centrally fixated (i.e., foveal) stationary target presented at a defined
distance. It can be measured with corrective lenses (corrected acuity) or without
(uncorrected acuity). When measured with the observer’s best optical correction for the
test distance, it is referred to as “best visual acuity” (BVA). Measurement is usually done
separately for each eye (monocular acuity) but can also be measured for both eyes
together (binocular acuity). Due to probability summation, binocular acuity is typically
15 to 20% better than monocular acuity.
Acuity is typically measured using size-graded rows of high-contrast “optotypes” (see
Figure 3.1). The observer’s task is to either identify the target on each row (e.g., Snellen
or Sloane letters), or to indicate the orientation of a repeated target form (e.g., bars or
gratings, “lazy Es”, or Landolt Cs). Acuity measures are thus indicative of an observer’s
ability to resolve fine spatial details, such as the text on a distant object (e.g., sign, ship
name, aircraft number) or small alphanumeric characters or symbols on instrument panel
displays and controls.
Figure 3.1. Examples of high-contrast acuity targets (Optotypes).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
11
Far acuity is usually measured at optical infinity for the eye (i.e., at 20 ft or 6 m) and is
usually denoted using one of two equivalent notations. When expressed using the
common “Snellen fraction” (e.g., 20/40), the “numerator” refers to the test distance in
feet and the “denominator” to the distance in feet at which an observer with “good”
acuity could resolve a critical target detail of the same size. The metric version of the
Snellen fraction expresses the corresponding information in meters (e.g., 6/12).
Acuity can also be expressed more directly in terms of the minimum visual angle
subtended by the smallest resolvable critical feature (e.g., the gap in a Landolt C), usually
in minutes of arc (minarc or arcmin). An acuity level of 1.0 minarc is equivalent to
Snellen 20/20 (6/6 metric), and 2.0 minarc to 20/40 Snellen (6/12 metric). Measured
under ideal viewing conditions, the best human acuity is about 20/10 (6/3 metric) or 0.5
minarc.
Acuity is a common and highly useful measure of spatial visual function for several
reasons. It can be easily measured by personnel with limited training using a wide
selection of readily available tests and it is quite reliable despite potentially significant
deviations from standard clinical conditions (e.g., Hawkins, 1995). It also provides an
excellent basis for clinical refraction (i.e., optical lens prescription) and is sensitive to
several optical (e.g., cataracts) and sensorineural disorders (e.g., macular degeneration).
Acuity is, however, affected by a wide range of stimulus and observer variables. It is
degraded by dim lighting, low target contrast, target crowding, target motion relative to
the observer and oblique target orientation. It is also subject to the effects of observer
age, pupil size, monocular versus binocular viewing and quality of optical correction (i.e.,
blur).
Good acuity, along with colour vision, is mediated by the cone photoreceptors of the
retina that are functional at relatively high (i.e., photopic) levels of illumination.
Consequently, low stimulus luminance impairs both of these visual functions. For a
young observer with good visual health, acuity can improve with target luminance up to
about 350 cd/m
2
, a level equivalent to good interior lighting. Due to the progressive
decline with age in retinal illuminance associated with a decline in resting pupil size
(senile miosis) and increased opacity of the lens of the eye, the luminance range over
which the acuity of older observers can benefit is considerably extended. Conversely,
poor viewing conditions tend to exacerbate the acuity problems of older observers as well
as those of any age with refractive problems such as myopia, hyperopia and astigmatism.
Optical blur, low luminance and low target/background contrast are known to degrade
acuity. However, their interactive effects in affecting performance on different visual
tasks are not as well understood. Johnson and Casson (1995) evaluated the potential
interactions of these three variables by comparing the Landolt C acuity of trained
psychophysical observers (each with 20/20 acuity or better) over five levels of contrast (6
to 97%), four levels of luminance (.075 cd/m
2
to 75 cd/m
2
) and nine levels of positive
sphere blur (0 to 8 D). They found that the effects of the blur, low luminance and
reduced contrast eroded acuity in an additive rather than interactive fashion. This led
Vision Standards for Aircrew
12
them to conclude that their data could allow for the prediction of acuity–mediated
performance in realistic viewing conditions. For example, they predicted that an
individual with 20/20 acuity at high luminance would decline to 20/60 in low luminance
and under conditions of both low luminance and low contrast, to 20/100.
3.1.2 Literature Review
The current standard, outlined by the Air Standardization Coordination Committee
(ASCC, 2003) for the uncorrected distance acuity of Canadian Forces pilots is 6/6 in the
better eye and 6/9 in the other eye. This is somewhat higher than that of most other
ASCC nations. For example, the corresponding standard for uncorrected acuity in
Australia is 6/12 in each eye (each eye correctable to 6/6), in New Zealand, 6/9 in each
eye (each correctable to 6/6) and in the United States, 6/21 in each eye (each correctable
to 6/6). Among ASCC countries, only the U.K. has a more demanding uncorrected
acuity standard than Canada (6/6 uncorrected each eye).
Meeting an initial entry acuity standard does not mean that an observer’s acuity will not
later decline to a level below the established standard due to visual disease and/or normal
aging. There is a well-documented decline in acuity with age. A small loss in
uncorrected acuity may be evident in individuals in their 30s or earlier, even among
relatively select observers (e.g., Gittings & Fozard, 1986). The decline in best corrected
acuity among healthy, well-corrected observers is likely to be noticeable much later, not
until approximately 50 to 60 years of age (e.g., Elliott, Yang, & Whitaker, 1995). In less
select “epidemiological” populations, age-related acuity declines tend to be more
significant (e.g., Attebo, Mitchell, & Smith, 1996; Klein, Klein, Lee, Cruickshanks &
Chappell, 2001).
It is also clear that military personnel selected for good acuity are not immune to age-
related visual deterioration. In response to lowering the uncorrected acuity standard for
Japan Air Self Defense Force personnel, Kikukawa, Yagura and Akamatsu (1999)
studied the distance acuity of 752 non-aviation personnel from age 20 to 45, 94% of
whom met the entry standard for student pilots. The proportion of the sample needing
corrective lenses increased with age from 15.8% to 37.1%. Over the 25 year time-frame
of the study, pilots with the best initial acuity showed a smaller decline in acuity and also
less need for corrective lenses than those with worse initial acuity. The authors noted that
lowering the initial standard was associated with an elevated risk of visual acuity loss as
the pilots aged. Presumably, careful and systematic visual and medical screening could
ameliorate this risk. That, at least, is the implication of a study by Miura, Shoji,
Fukumoto, Yasue, Tsukui and Hosoya (2002). They found that increasing the allowable
age from 60 to 63 years for Japanese airline transport pilots did not appear to be
associated with any decline in safety, a result that they attributed to the medical screening
regimen. Similarly, while Eyraud and Borowsky (1985) found that the profile of accident
types changed with age for fighter, attack and helicopter pilots, the overall accident rate
for pilots 37 to 47 years of age did not differ significantly from that of pilots aged 22 to
37.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
13
Changes in acuity after entry into the military, whether due to aging or some other
cause(s), highlight the need for ongoing screening if visual readiness is to be maintained.
A study of the prevalence of substandard acuity of 207 members of several different
communities in the U.S. Air Force (Erneston, Ricks, Tate, & Ana, 1996) found that 54%
had not had a professional eye exam in the previous two years, 24% were not mobility
ready, 3% had inadequate acuity relative to the standard and 1.9% had ocular disease.
A larger scale study (Buckingham, Cornforth, Whitwell & Lee, 2003) found problems of
visual readiness to be even more prevalent in the different branches of the U.S military
(USA, USAF, USMC, USN). Of the 4825 active duty personnel tested, 83.3% were not
vision ready, 10.4% had substandard acuity and 73.8% had eye-health related
deficiencies. There was, however, considerable variability in these data across the
different service branches. The prevalence of personnel who were deemed not visual
acuity ready ranged from a low of 3.5% in the USAF to a high of 15.4% in the USN.
Far acuity is a measure of an observer’s ability to resolve fine detail at distance and it is
predictive of performance on a wide range of everyday visual tasks, such as Horton,
performed by aircrew. For example, acuity is related to face recognition (e.g., Bullimore,
Bailey, & Wacker, 1991), identifying suspicious behaviour or determining if a person is
carrying a weapon (e.g., Good & Ausberger, 1987; Johnson, Casson & Zadnik, 1992) and
reading distant text materials such as license plates (e.g., Kiel, Butler, & Alwitry, 2003;
Sheedy, 1980) or road signs (e.g., Horton & Joseph, 2002).
Acuity has also been related to performance on a range of tasks in the marine
environment including the identification of marker buoys (Donderi, Kawaja, Smiley,
Henderson, & Zadra, 1994), the detection and identification of navigation lights, the
detection of a simulated man-over-board and the detection of ship-to-ship signals
(Casson, Gibbs & Cameron, 1999b). In the latter task, performance fell to near-chance
levels when acuity was degraded to 20/40 (6/12). Even a small reduction in acuity has
been shown to impair the detection of a life raft in daylight search and rescue (e.g.,
Donderi, 1994).
3.1.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified a number of common pilot tasks that are challenging to
the visual system in terms of far visual acuity. Target identification tasks (for example
reading ship lettering from a far distance in order to identify the ship type and allegiance,
or identifying other aircraft as friendly or enemy) were reported to be challenging far
acuity tasks. This is due to the inherent risk associated with closing proximity to an
enemy ship, aircraft or other armed vehicle. The further away a pilot determines key
aspects of a target, such as determining friendly or enemy status, direction of heading or
orientation, the safer a pilot will remain. SMEs also reported the need to be able to see
runway hazards and distant air traffic. For example, one SME recalled an incident in
which he saw a small private plane crossing into his flight path that he was able to avoid,
due to dependable far acuity.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
14
3.2 Near Acuity
3.2.1 Description
Like its far counterpart, near acuity refers to the ability to resolve fine details usually in
high-contrast stimuli. Near acuity is basically a measure of the visual resolution needed
for reading and similar close tasks and is conventionally measured and corrected (as
needed) at normal reading distance (16 in. or 40 cm). Although near acuity is often
specified using the familiar “20/N” Snellen notation, technically, like far acuity, it should
be specified in terms of the distance at which it is measured (e.g., as 16/32 not 20/40, or
in metric terms as 40/80 rather than 6/12). This issue highlights the advantage of
specifying both far and near acuity in terms of minarc, where 1.0 minarc specifies the
same angular resolution for both far Snellen (20/20 or 6/6) and near Snellen (16/16 or
40/40).
‘N-notation’ is another system that is used to specify near acuity, especially for reading
text. N charts are composed of continuous “paragraphs” printed in a Times New Roman
font that ranges from smallest size (N5 - corresponding roughly to a resolution of 1.6
minarc or a Snellen near acuity of 16/26) to the largest (N60 - about 19.2 minarc or
16/307). There is a direct mathematical relationship between N size and font size (e.g.,
the N12 font is half the size of N24 and twice the size of N6). It should be noted, that to
read at an optimal rate, print should be about twice the observer’s acuity threshold
(Whittaker & Lovie-Kitchen, 1993).
Near acuity is also affected by many of the same variables as far acuity (i.e., lighting,
target contrast, crowding, motion, orientation, observer age, pupil size, monocular versus
binocular viewing, and optical correction). Uncorrected near acuity, however, is
particularly vulnerable to the effects of ocular aging. Near stimuli demand more
refractive (accommodative) power from the lens of the eye; a function that declines more
or less linearly with age from its peak at about 10 years of age. By age 60, due to
sclerosis in the lens and possibly changes in the ciliary muscle, virtually all
accommodative amplitude is lost in both the general (c.f., Kline & Scialfa, 1996) and
medically-screened pilot populations (e.g., Szafran, 1969).
The associated recession of the near point of vision, known as presbyopia, is often
noticeable by approximately 40 years of age, at which time the observer is likely to need
an optical “add” to provide the increase in optical power (e.g., bifocal, trifocal or
progressive lenses) to focus near stimuli. As the aging eye progresses toward a “fixed-
focus” state, the observer’s optical correction will increasingly determine the ability to
focus on items at different distances. Trifocals have a near (40 cm), far (6 m) and
intermediate distance correction (depending on the prescription, usually between 50 and
100 cm). The refractive power of progressive lenses increases toward the lower near
segment and they provide a relatively continuous distance correction, although they
demand good alignment of the gaze through the spectacle relative to display distance.
With a well-prescribed bifocal, the wearer is corrected for near and far but not
intermediate viewing distances such as those characteristic of instrument panels.
Depending on the accommodative ability that remains, the instrument panel may be out
Vision Standards for Aircrew
15
of focus (i.e., blurred), or cause eyestrain if extended accommodative exertion (i.e., more
than half the observer’s remaining accommodative reserve) is needed to achieve focus.
The problems of blur and eye-strain for observers with low accommodative capacity are
depicted graphically in Figure 3.2 as a function of age and display distance. It shows
how a good optical correction becomes increasingly essential for an older pilot, even one
with excellent far acuity. This issue has received longstanding attention in the
aviation/vision research literature (e.g., Backman, & Dow-Smith, 1975; Markovits,
Reddix, O’Connell, & Collyer, 1995).
Figure 3.2. The effects of age-related accommodative loss on focus and eye-strain with
extended viewing for observers with low-accommodation. Source: Kline, Caird, Ho, &
Dewar (2002).
3.2.2 Literature Review
The near acuity standard for CF aircrew (ASCC, 2003) is specified in N-notation form
for both reading distance and 100 cm. For the better eye, the standard is N5 at reading
distance and N14 at 100 cm; for the worse eye, N6 is specified at reading distance and
N18 at 100 cm. Presumably, the 100-cm distance is specified to indicate the need for
good acuity at intermediate cockpit instrument display distances.
Effective near and intermediate-distance acuity is essential for a wide range of
operational tasks in aviation and related environments, including reading navigational
charts and control system labels, and monitoring flight systems. Reduced acuity impairs
the ability of Coast Guard employees to carry out near clerical tasks similar to those for
sea-going personnel (Donderi, Kawaja, Smiley, Henderson, & Zadra, 1994) and also
Vision Standards for Aircrew
16
degrades the ability of avionics technicians to read printed wire codes in the electrical
compartment of large aircraft (Casson, Gibbs, & Cameron, 1999a).
Optically degraded near acuity has also been shown to affect pilot cockpit performance.
Mann and Hovis (1996) examined the ability of 15 instrument-rated pilots to carry out
simulator instrument-flight-rules (IFR) approaches under four levels of optically
degraded acuity. Although most of the pilots were able to maintain decision height under
all blur levels due to the vernier acuity-like nature of the task display, localizer positions
were degraded during the inbound phase in daytime lighting. The major effect of
degraded acuity was to impair the pilots’ ability to read approach plates and to set proper
radio frequencies. Under daylight conditions, approximately 30% of the participants
could not read the approach plates with a near acuity of .50 logMAR (about 3 minarc or
16/50). This finding is similar to that of Draeger, Brandl, Wirt, and Burchard (1989) who
found that acuity below 6/7 hindered pilots’ ability to read charts and maps accurately at
the time of approach. The importance of near acuity for this task is significant,
considering the numerous small details that must be resolved on approach plates (see
Figure 3.3).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
17
Figure 3.3. Approach plate for Greater Moncton International Airport – Atlantic Region.
Source: Geomatics Canada, Dept of Natural Resources, 2005.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
18
3.2.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of near visual acuity. One task that is common across all aircraft is
reading approach plates in the cockpit. The SMEs indicated that approach plates have
become increasingly challenging to read and decipher due to the constant updating of
flight information, which in turn, clutter the plates. This task can be made further
challenging when combined with a vibrating cockpit, or while wearing Night Vision
Goggles (NVGs). Other challenging near visual acuity tasks performed by pilots include
reading Visual Flight Rules (VFR) maps and contour maps, viewing CRT
instrumentation and the weather radar display and interaction with the flight management
system. Depending on the aircraft, this may have to be done under green light, blue-
filtered light or red light. One SME recalled a situation in which an older pilot had
difficulty reading an approach plate and noted that in such situations the co-pilot (in
multi-crew aircraft) will often take responsibility for checklist and reading tasks.
3.3 Contrast Sensitivity (CS)
3.3.1 Description
Stimuli in the natural environment are not generally high in luminance contrast relative to
their background. In recognition of this, basic and clinical research increasingly
examines the possibility that low-contrast measures may be more sensitive than high-
contrast tests for identifying many eye health problems and/or predicting task
performance in viewing conditions affected by stimulus contrast. In general, studies
support this view, showing that some observers who are able to identify crisp black and
white targets on a regular acuity chart may have inordinate difficulty in seeing at night, in
a dimly lit room, or in glare. It has been documented that CS measures are better than
visual acuity in predicting success when detecting and identifying common objects
(Owsley & Sloane, 1987), in discriminating highway signs (Evans & Ginsburg, 1985)
and for understanding the everyday visual problems of aging drivers (Schieber, Kline,
Kline, & Fozard, 1992).
Several different CS test systems have been developed to determine observer sensitivity
(1/contrast threshold) to low contrast stimuli. Some of these tests are analogous to acuity
charts in using letters, but they differ in that contrast sensitivity rather than minimum
stimulus size is measured. For example, the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart (Pelli,
Robson & Wilkins, 1988) shown in Figure 3.4, uses fairly large letters of a constant size
that decrease in contrast for each group of three letters from the top to bottom of the
chart. Normally presented at three meters, this test primarily measures the ability to
discern contrast in large (i.e., low spatial frequency) stimuli. As implied by its name, the
Small Letter Contrast Test (SLCT) uses small letters (equivalent to 20/25) across 14 lines
of decreasing contrast to test CS at four meters (e.g., Rabin, 1996). The Regan low-
contrast letter test (Regan & Neima, 1983) is composed of a series of charts each with
letters of different size but of systematically varied contrast. As a result, it assesses
contrast sensitivity over a broader domain of spatial size than the Pelli-Robson or SLCT
charts.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
19
Figure 3.4. A sample Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Pelli, D. G., Robson, J. G., &
Wilkins, A. J., 1988. Copyright © 2002 D.G. Pelli and J.G. Robson. Distributed by Haag-
Streit.
Letter charts for testing CS have the virtues of familiarity for both the administrator and
patient, often require little special equipment and are also quickly and easily scored.
They are, however, generally less precise and less comprehensive than tests based on
sinusoidal luminance gratings (e.g., Fig. 3.5). By assessing an observer’s ability to
discriminate luminance contrast differences in sinusoidal gratings of varied spatial
frequency (i.e., grating fineness), the contrast sensitivity function (CSF) provides a
comprehensive measure of spatial vision ability. Numerous computer-based grating-
Vision Standards for Aircrew
20
based CS testing systems of this type exist, but most are custom to different labs and are
not standardized in testing method, spatial frequency, or testing conditions.
A few systems, such as the Functional Acuity Contrast Test (FACT –
www.contrastsensitivity.net) device and its predecessor, the chart-based (near and far)
Vistech Contrast Testing System (VCTS - Vistech Consultants, 1988), however, have
been developed for more general use. On Vistech charts (see Figure 3.5), spatial
frequency increases from the top to bottom row and contrast decreases from left to the
right. Sensitivity for each spatial frequency is based on the rightmost (i.e., lowest-
contrast) grating for which orientation is correctly reported.
Figure 3.5. Sample VCTS 6500 contrast sensitivity chart. Source: Vistech Consultants
(1988).
In most studies, age effects on the CSF are generally negligible at low spatial frequencies,
but an emerging age deficit is seen for gratings of intermediate and higher spatial
frequency (e.g., Elliott, Whitaker, & MacVeigh, 1990; Kline, Schieber, Abusamra &
Coyne, 1983; Owsley Sekuler, & Siemsen, 1983). The extent of prior CS loss also
appears to be a robust predictor of later acuity loss in older observers (Schneck,
Haegerstrom-Portnoy, Lotta, Brabyn, & Gildengorin, 2004).
3.3.2 Literature Review
Sensitivity to contrast appears to be particularly useful for predicting performance in the
presence of optical aberrations, glare and dim light. For example, scores on the SLCT
appear to be more sensitive than acuity, to both subtle optical defocus (Rabin, 1994) and
reduced target luminance (Rabin, 1995). CS and low-contrast acuity are highly sensitive
to glare effects with cataracts (Elliott, & Bullimore, 1993) and CS is more sensitive than
high-contrast acuity to optical degradation associated with photorefractive surgery (e.g.,
Vision Standards for Aircrew
21
Verdon, Maloney, & Bullimore, 1995). Sensitivity to low contrast also appears to
enhance effective performance on a wide range of real-world tasks, including many in
aviation. This includes sensitivity to the optical effects of aircraft windscreens on
visibility, especially in glare (Hughes & Vingrys, 1991). It has been shown to be more
strongly correlated than acuity with performance on a simulated search and rescue task
(Stager & Hameluck, 1986). CS also appears to be superior to conventional acuity
measures for predicting a pilot’s ability to detect small air-to-ground targets in an aircraft
simulator (Ginsburg, Evans, Sekuler & Harp, 1982) as well as target detection in the field
(Ginsburg & Easterly, 1983).
Numerous studies have found CS measures to be related to performance on a diverse
range of aircrew tasks (e.g., Rabin, 1995) and significant levels of research have been
devoted to the development of CS measures for aircrew screening (e.g., Gray &
McFadden, 1987; McFadden & Kaufmann, 1993; Grimson, Schallhorn & Kaupp, 2002;
Swamy, Joseph, Aravind & Vevai, 2002). No standard has yet been set for CS in military
aviation, however, nor will one be feasible without further validation research. Different
CS tests produce highly variant results (e.g., Elliott & Whitaker, 1992; Hitchcock, Dick
& Krieg, 2004; McFadden, 1994), they often lack acceptable test-retest reliability
(Pesudovs, Hazel, Doran, Elliott, 2004) and general population norms are not suitable for
application to the visually-select aircrew community (Swamy, Joseph, Aravind, & Vevai,
2002). The effort to establish a CS standard may be encouraged especially when
considering the results of a study conducted to establish large-group normative data for
screening student naval pilots using the SLCT (Grimson, Schallhorn, & Kaupp, 2002).
The naval student pilots (N = 107) scored significantly better than a military control
population that included both aviation (N=366) and non-aviation personnel (N=185),
leading the authors to conclude that the SLCT showed potential for use as a screening test
during the induction exam of military pilots.
3.3.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
acuity system in terms of contrast sensitivity. Target identification in conditions of low
contrast (for example identifying a grey ship silhouette against sea water in order to
identify the ship type and allegiance) was reported to be a challenging low contrast acuity
task. Similarly, when flying an aircraft into a forested area, the pilot must determine the
tallest tree and its relation to the aircraft, as this will affect the chosen flight path and
altitude. However, determining the tallest tree is difficult when there is very little
contrast differentiating trees from one another. This task is made even more difficult
when performed under sunset or dusk conditions, as the haze associated with such times
lowers all contrast detail.
Another common pilot task that challenges the visual system in terms of contrast
sensitivity is identifying targets in the Search and Rescue (SAR) environment. For
example, finding a white aircraft in the snow, or a green tank in a forest are both difficult
tasks due to the lack of contrast between the targets and their respective backgrounds.
One SME recalled a time when he was tasked to go to a crash site and was provided with
its exact position, but he could not find the site as it blended in so well with the
Vision Standards for Aircrew
22
surrounding forest environment. He eventually found the site when sunlight, reflected
from part of the vehicle for which he was searching for, attracted his attention.
3.4 Visual Fields and Useful Field of View
3.4.1 Visual Fields
3.4.1.1 Description
The visual field is a measurement of the spatial extent of vision. Depending on the
facilities available to a lab or clinic, the field is usually measured in one of three ways.
The confrontational exam is a quick evaluation of the visual field performed by an
examiner sitting directly in front of an observer. With one eye covered, the observer may
be asked to look at the examiner's eye and indicate the locations at which they can see the
examiner’s hand. On the tangent-screen exam, the observer fixates a central target and
informs the examiner when he/she can see an object entering the peripheral vision along
various orientations. In automated perimetry, the observer is seated in front of a concave
dome and fixates on a central target. A computer-driven program flashes small lights at
different locations within the dome and the observer presses a switch to indicate the lights
that are seen. These responses are compared to age-matched norms to determine the
presence of defects within the visual field. For a visually healthy human observer, the
horizontal visual field for one eye may extend 100
0
to110
0
temporally and 60
0
nasally.
Due to binocular overlap, this produces a full horizontal field extent of about 180
0
.
Vertically, the inferior field may extend to 70 to 75
0
and the superior field to about 60
0
.
Overall field size may contract due to an eye disease (e.g., glaucoma, retinitis
pigmentosa). There can also be “holes” in the visual field due to eye injury (e.g., solar
damage, detached retina) or disease (e.g., macular degeneration, diabetic retinopathy).
3.4.1.2 Literature Review
Peripheral vision plays a critical role in visually guided behavior (e.g, walking, driving,
low-level flying, etc.) and in directing attention in surveillance and search tasks. For this
reason, all ASCC nations specify a visual field standard for military flying (ASCC,
2003). For CF aircrew, the visual field can be established initially by the confrontational
method and if the need is indicated, by follow-up automated perimetry using the
Humphries VF device. Loss of peripheral vision (not defined) or the presence of scotoma
are both cause for candidate rejection.
Visual changes or conditions that limit the information available from the peripheral
visual field have been shown to degrade performance on a range of tasks involving
mobility and field search. A large-scale study that evaluated the visual fields of 10,000
drivers (Johnson & Keltner, 1983), found that binocular field losses were associated with
accident and violation rates more than double those of age- and sex-matched control
participants. The accident and violation rates of drivers with field losses in only one eye,
however, were not elevated.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
23
Some simulator studies have shown that field losses due to eye disease produce a
decrement in driving performance (e.g., Hedin & Lovsund, 1987; Szlyk, Brigell & Seiple,
1993). However, more recent studies (e.g., Myers, Ball, Kalina, Roth & Goode, 2000)
indicate that current perimetry tests are not strong predictors of an individual’s driving
performance relative to the useful field of view test (see section 3.4.2 following).
Due to the 3-D open field nature of the aviation environment, intact visual fields may be
even more critical for some flying tasks than for driving. For example, in daytime low-
level flight, visual flow provides critical information about speed and altitude (Foyle,
Kaiser, & Johnson, 1992). The field of vision also appears to affect estimates of closing
speed to collision with (for example) another helicopter in a simulator setting (Kruk, &
Regan, 1996). Given the importance of peripheral vision in the cockpit, it has been
suggested that the field losses due to glaucoma represent a potential problem for pilots
due to the relatively long interval between exams (Schwartz, Stern, Klemm, Draeger, &
Winter, 1996). It may also be important to distinguish the effects of field losses initially
from those observed after an adaptation period. For example, there is evidence that the
effects of visual field reduction due to monocular loss can be compensated for on some
tasks, if the pilot has sufficient opportunity to adapt to the loss (Kochhar & Fraser, 1978).
3.4.1.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of visual fields. The pilots indicated that some of the cockpit warning
lights are located primarily in the peripheral field of vision (FOV), rather than the direct
field of view. Depending upon the aircraft, a warning light may be blinking, flashing,
and/or may have an auditory component (not all alarms in the peripheral field of view
have an auditory component). Therefore, peripheral vision is extremely important in
detecting and identifying cockpit warning lights. Further, the pilots reported that NVGs
have a 40-degree FOV that severely limits their viewing capability of cockpit warning
lights.
Pilots also reported that they use their peripheral FOV to detect birds, other aircraft and to
determine depth perception. Similarly, the peripheral FOV is important for stability
while flying. It is used by pilots to ascertain where they are in comparison to the horizon
and indicates whether the aircraft is level in comparison to the horizon (determining
attitude: pitch, roll and yaw). This task is more difficult to perform at night without the
visual cues provided by the horizon.
Related to this, landing on a ship at night is a difficult task, considering there are few
peripheral FOV cues available. In this instance, a pilot may rely on the co-pilot to
provide information related to depth and proximity to the ship. Personnel on the ship
may also drop flares, with the intent of aiding pilot vision (although this can actually
hinder pilot vision with the complete loss of their night vision adaptation- See Section 3.7
Night Vision).
One situation the helicopter pilots must try to avoid by using their peripheral vision is the
prevention of a ‘Snowball’ effect when landing the helicopter. The pilots must try to
Vision Standards for Aircrew
24
prevent stirring up elements with the helicopter (such as snow, sand, dust, etc) or they
may lose their references to external cues such as altitude and aircraft angle in relation to
the ground. The peripheral FOV is necessary for detecting a snowball forming adjacent
to the aircraft door, which acts as a cue to the pilot to increase speed before visibility is
lost.
3.4.2 Useful Field of View
3.4.2.1 Description
The Useful Field of View (UFOV) refers to the ability to locate, identify and discriminate
visual stimuli presented so briefly as to preclude eye movements. A higher order task
than simple field size, it measures selective and divided attention as well as rapid visual
processing. In its usual form, the UFOV measure asks observers to identify a central
stimulus while also locating a peripherally located target in the presence of distracter
elements. The size of the UFOV varies with display characteristics and task demands. It
is reduced when attentional demands are increased by the addition of a secondary
discrimination task in central vision (Ball, Beard, Roenker, Miller & Griggs, 1988) or
when the number of background distracter stimuli is increased (Scialfa, Kline & Lyman,
1987).
Performance on UFOV tasks declines with age (e.g., Ball, Roenker, & Bruni, 1990), a
change that appears to reflect a reduction in the efficiency with which information can be
extracted from complex scenes. This loss is exacerbated when attention must be divided
between a central and peripheral task (e.g., Sekuler, Bennett, & Mamelak, 2000). Among
older drivers, the UFOV appears to account for nearly 20% of crash variance in both
retrospective and prospective studies (see Ball, Owsley, Sloane, Roenker & Bruni, 1993).
Also, studies in simulated traffic environments have shown that prolonged driving can
cause a contraction in the UFOV (Rogé, Pebayle, Hannachi, & Muzet, 2003). A similar
contraction in the spatial extent of visual attention has been attributed to the “cognitive-
capture” effects of head-up displays in aircraft (Prinzel, 2004).
3.4.2.2 Literature Review
Although the research literature on the predictive utility of UFOV for driving safety and
performance is growing rapidly, there seems to be very little comparable work in the field
of civil or military aviation (searches for “UFOV and pilots”, “UFOV and flying, “UFOV
and aviation” and similar terms returned no citations). As a result, the potential of the
UFOV test for screening aircrew remains largely unexplored. Considering that peripheral
visual feedback seems to be highly effective for supporting attention in event-driven data-
rich environments such as modern aircraft (Nikolic & Sarter, 2001), the utility of higher-
order peripheral vision assessment techniques in aviation is likely to increase in the
future.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
25
3.4.2.3 Related Tasks
In the fixed wing environment, the UFOV is important when taxiing the aircraft. In order
to maintain the aircraft on the runway, the UFOV is used to avoid for example, hangers
and other airplanes. A wide UFOV is also important in identifying the presence of birds
as well as the location of other aircraft in the landing order. Further, it is essential in
preventing movement illusions that may occur when performing single light approaches
to a ship or runway at night, or when performing hovering activities.
3.5 Glare Sensitivity & Recovery
3.5.1 Disability Glare
3.5.1.1 Description
When light from a strong “veiling” source falls onto the retina, it reduces image contrast.
The nearer the glare source is to the line of sight, the greater the problem. The effects of
such disability glare are usually measured in terms of the impact on acuity or contrast
sensitivity. Although disability glare can occur during daytime (e.g., flying into the sun
early or late in the day, sunlight reflected from snow-covered terrain) it is even more
prevalent in low-luminance conditions (e.g., bright lights at night). The problems
associated with disability glare generally increase with age, primarily because the
senescent lens of an older viewer scatters light to a greater degree than its younger
counterpart (e.g., Brabyn, Haegerstroem-Portnoy, Schneck, & Lott, 2000; Guirao,
Gonzalez, Redondo, Geraghty, Norrby & Artal, 1999).
Anderson and Holliday (1995) measured the effects of car headlight glare (off, low beam
and high beam) on motion direction discrimination with blurred vision and simulated
intraocular lens opacities. Simulated opacities that had little or no effect on daytime
static acuity, significantly reduced contrast sensitivity for moving targets and led the
authors to suggest that driver visual screening should include testing under nighttime
driving conditions. The effects of disability glare are likely to be even more critical in
military aviation.
3.5.1.2 Literature Review
A variety of lighting sources, including sunlight, fire, flares, explosions, even on-board
camera flash systems (e.g., McFadden, 1982), can produce disability glare in aviation.
There is also evidence that glare from the sun has contributed to accidents in civil
aviation, most frequently during day time clear-weather approaches and take-offs
(Nakagawara, Wood, & Montgomery, 2004). Glare has been shown to reduce nighttime
on-road driving performance and pedestrian detection, especially that of older drivers
(Theeuwes, Alferdinck & Perel, 2002). Relatively little research, however, has been
devoted to studying the effects of glare on various aircrew tasks and no standard
disability-glare resistance standard has been set for pilot entry into the air service in any
of the ASCC countries. This is not surprising given the considerable inter-individual
variability in susceptibility to disability glare, even among pilot groups (Temme, Still, &
Vision Standards for Aircrew
26
Fatcheric, 1995) and that there are dozens of different systems but few standard
methodologies for testing it.
Elliott and Bullimore (1993) compared the reliability, discriminative ability and validity
of five different glare testing systems (the Miller-Nadler, Vistech MCT8000, Berkeley,
van den Berg Straylightmeter, and Brightness Acuity Tester (BAT) used with Pelli-
Robson and Regan charts). They found that contrast sensitivity and low-contrast acuity
from the Pelli-Robson, Regan and Bekeley tests provided similar reliable, discriminative
and valid measures of cataract and concluded that without decent chart design and
psychophysical methods, the design and geometry of the glare source are of little
importance. However, when Tan, Spalton and Arden (1998) compared the
Straylightmeter and BAT before and after removal of posterior cataracts, they found that
the glare testing in combination with acuity provided more information than contrast
sensitivity and that glare effects were better assessed by the Straylightmeter in
comparison to the BAT.
3.5.1.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of glare sensitivity and recovery, specifically, disability glare. All of the
SMEs reported that they experience disability glare when taking off and landing on a
runway directly facing the sun (sunset or sunrise). Bright sunshine also inhibits the
ability to read aircraft instrumentation and disability glare is a problem for pilots flying
over water in the sunshine. It can also make formation flying and judging the direction of
other aircraft travel very difficult. Some aircraft have flip-down glare shields that the
pilots may use to alleviate glare sources (although these only work well if the aircraft is
flown in one direction, as they typically make the view too dark in the direction opposite
to the sun).
One SME reported disability glare associated with landing on a hospital pad at night.
Very strong lights were shining on the landing pad. These lights were blindingly bright
in comparison to the dark surroundings, which in turn inhibited the pilot’s dark
adaptation and peripheral vision.
3.5.2 Discomfort Glare
3.5.2.1 Description
Discomfort glare refers to light effects that are annoying but do not necessarily interfere
with task performance. A wide variety of sources can produce discomfort glare,
including lighting fixtures, headlights, strong luminance differences between adjacent
surfaces as well as reflections from snow, windows, CRT screens, windshields and
aircraft canopies. Discomfort increases directly with the intensity of the source and
inversely with the angle between the glare source(s) and the line of sight. Discomfort
glare also tends to be greater for flashing or scintillating light sources than for stable
sources (King, 1972). Primarily a psychological phenomenon (Saur, 1969), discomfort
glare is measured by subjective rating.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
27
3.5.2.2 Literature Review
Sensitivity to discomfort glare appears to increase with observer age in the general
population (e.g., Hughes & Neer, 1981). This may also be applicable to military pilots,
but little research has been devoted to the topic.
3.5.2.3 Related Tasks
SMEs reported some discomfort glare is experienced when approaching bright runway
lights from a dark environment. It was also reported for the flashbulb effect from mortar
flashes and for ground illumination in the vicinity of an aerodrome.
3.5.3 Glare Recovery
3.5.3.1 Description
Due to the protracted recovery of retinal sensitivity after exposure to a strong transient
glare source (e.g., oncoming vehicle headlights at night), visibility can be adversely
affected well after the initial exposure. Bichao, Yager and Meng (1995) have shown that
exposure to transient glare raised discrimination thresholds by .5 to .75 log units more
than a steady source, an effect that was even more pronounced and enduring in the visual
periphery. Further to this, older observers take longer to recover from exposure to a
strong transient glare source (e.g., Carter, 1994; Elliott & Whitaker, 1990). This is
particularly true if the targeted viewing object is low in contrast relative to its
background. When Schieber (1994) measured the ability to identify low-contrast letter
pairs after exposure to glare, he found that older observers required three-times longer to
recover than did their younger counterparts.
3.5.3.2 Literature Review
One study found protracted glare recovery time to be associated with an increased risk of
automobile accidents (Roy & Choudhary, 1985), while the results of another study
indicate that glare recovery can be predicted by low contrast acuity (Schneck, et al.,
2004). There is little research in the public domain on the effects of glare recovery time
on performance of aviation tasks (as assessed by online searches for glare recovery and
scotomatic glare). As for disability and discomfort glare, no standard method has been
accepted for measuring glare recovery.
3.5.3.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of recovery from glare specifically, transient sources. Flying directly
into bright sunshine and then changing direction to have the sun located directly behind
the aircraft, was a problem noted by all SMEs. Of specific concern was the long recovery
time required after exposure to glare. SMEs indicated that glare shields and sunglasses
help to alleviate this problem. Similarly, when taking off from a runway at night, pilots
may be surrounded by bright lights and then quite suddenly enveloped in complete
darkness. This inhibits dark adaptation to the outside environment as well as the ability
to read and interact with important elements within the cockpit (for example, approach
Vision Standards for Aircrew
28
plates, aircraft instrumentation, etc.). Glare is also a problem when using paraflares to
aid in landing or when flying in the ‘flashbulb’ effect from mortar fire. When attempting
to land in the cover of darkness, if a paraflare is used it provides immediate bright light
and then complete darkness again (similar to mortar fire), inhibiting the visual system.
Finally, the SMEs reported that glare was a concern when flying in and out of cloud
cover when it is inter-mixed with bright sunshine.
3.6 Colour Vision
3.6.1 Description
An observer’s ability to process colour information determines the contribution that
colour contrast can make to form perception. Normal trichromatic colour vision allows
an observer to discriminate colour differences in the full spectral range. However, colour
discrimination can be difficult or even impossible for those with colour vision
deficiencies unless the information is conveyed redundantly (e.g., by shape, size,
temporal property, or brightness). Colour deficiencies are generally distinguished
depending on whether they are congenital (present at, or soon after birth), or acquired
(develop as a consequence of environmental exposure, disease or trauma).
There are three types of congenital colour vision deficiency, monochromacy, dichromacy
and anomalous trichromacy. Monochromats, due to the absence of cone photoreceptors,
are truly “colour blind” and also have very poor acuity. Dichromats, having only two
types of cone photopigment, need only two wavelengths to match any wavelength so they
experience colour over a smaller range than do trichromats. The three forms of
dichromacy, protanopia, deuteranopia, and tritanopia, are labeled as to the types of cone
photopigment that is missing. Protanopia and deuteranopia, both sex-linked deficiencies,
are far more common among males than females. Both involve problems discriminating
red and green - short wavelengths are seen as blue and long wavelengths as yellow.
Tritanopia, which is quite rare, leads to a confusion of blues and yellows - greens and
reds are seen at short and long wavelengths, respectively. Like normal trichromats,
anomalous trichromats need three wavelengths to match any other wavelength, but they
use them in different proportions than those with normal colour vision. They may also
have more difficulty discriminating some wavelengths than those with normal
trichromatic colour perception, due to a convergent shift of the red and green pigment
spectra.
Colour vision deficiencies may also be acquired as the consequence of disease, trauma or
toxicity. Such deficiencies tend to be equally prevalent in males and females,
asymmetric between the two eyes and less stable over time than congenital deficiencies.
Acquired deficiencies are more likely to manifest as yellow-blue rather than red-green
defects. A wide range of causal factors can be involved including normal aging, diabetic
retinopathy, cataract, age-related maculopathy (ARM), and medications. Although it is
not a profound effect, colour discrimination tends to decline with age, more so for short
than long wavelengths (Kinnear & Sahraie, 2002).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
29
3.6.2 Literature Review
Consistent with the need for effective colour perception in aviation, the CF pilot entry
standard (ASCC, 2003) specifies a two-stage contingent testing protocol. Initial
screening is carried out using a pseudoisochromatic plate (PIP) test (e.g., the Ishihara)
and includes a Blue-Yellow plate (for tritan defect). If the score on the PIP is less than
17 out of 21, a Holmes-Wright or Farnsworth lantern test (FALANT) is administered.
Any errors on the lantern test leads to a designation of CV3, the minimum colour
standard that will allow dichromats and severe anomalous trichromats to enter the CF.
For the normal trichromatic observer, colour can be a powerful aid to a visual search and
identification task (e.g., Christ, 1975; D’Zmura, 1991). Bowman and Cole (1981) found
that observers with good colour vision and acuity were able to use colour-coded
navigation lights to facilitate determination of intruder aircraft orientation and direction.
Redundant colour coding has been shown to facilitate overall search speed on airborne
CRT displays (Luder & Barber, 1984) and to enhance speed and reduce errors on cockpit
identification/search tasks (Macdonald & Cole, 1988).
In contrast, deficient colour vision can impede performance on a diverse range of tasks
(e.g., Cole, 1993; Cole, 2004). Steward and Cole (1989) found that almost 90% of
dichromats and about two-thirds of anomalous trichromats reported difficulties with
everyday tasks that involve colour. Approximately half of the dichromats and
approximately 20% of the anomalous trichromats experience difficulty distinguishing
traffic lights and carrying out their jobs. O’Brien, Cole, Maddocks and Forbes (2002)
reported that deuteranopia reduced the attention conspicuity of traffic signs and signals.
Defective colour vision has also been shown to impede the acquisition of information
from redundantly coded video displays (Cole & Macdonald, 1988) and polychromatic
sonar screens (Scholz, Andresen, Hofmann, & Duncker, 1995).
Ishihara and Farnsworth Munsell colour vision tests have been shown to predict
performance on buoy identification at high illumination levels and engineering
performance (e.g., color naming, direction and colour of piping arrows) under low
illumination (Donderi, et al., 1994). Mertens and Milburn (1998) evaluated the predictive
validity of 13 colour vision tests by comparing the performance of colour-normal and
colour-deficient observers on three air traffic control tasks (flight progress strips, aircraft
lights and colour weather radar). They concluded that a high level of colour vision ability
was essential to accurate task performance; however, they also noted that three of the
tests evaluated had overly high false-alarm rates.
The available research presents a compelling case in favour of the need for colour vision
standards for occupational tasks where human observer colour judgment cannot be
supplanted by automation (Vingrys & Cole, 1986; Vingrys & Cole, 1988; Cole, 2004).
To be occupationally relevant, however, tests must be matched to the colour demands
encountered in operational conditions. In that regard, it should be noted that the Ishihara
PIP test, which can be administered in the first stage of colour testing for CF pilot entry,
may fail some observers who are colour-normal (Hovis & Oliphant, 2000). Conversely,
the Farnsworth lantern test, which can be administered in stage two, will fail observers
Vision Standards for Aircrew
30
with severe red-green colour vision deficiencies, but it does not allow a determination of
the type or severity of the colour deficiency (Birch & Dain, 1999).
3.6.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of colour vision. The VFR maps, approach plates, contour maps, CRT
instrumentation, weather radar, ground proximity warning system, flight management
system, runway approach lighting and runway lights all have associated colour coding.
For example, the VFR maps use colour to distinguish relief and contours.
The SMEs reported that the aircraft instrument panels are colour coded (for example,
weather radar) and yet there is no secondary or redundant coding associated with the
colour. This is important when considering that small changes in coloured lighting can
severely affect people with poor colour vision. For example, the Tactical Helicopter
pilots at times are required to fly with laser eye protection (a light green filter) that affects
all viewable colours and even completely washes out some colours.
The ability to distinguish colour is also important for target detection and identification.
The SMEs reported that SAR pilots must be able to identify (for example), an orange life
raft in the ocean, or white smoke on an ocean with whitecaps. Also, the ability to
determine green from red is extremely important, especially to determine the meaning of
a flare (normally colour coded), or to distinguish the direction of travel of other aircraft
by viewing their red or green navigation lights.
3.7 Night Vision
3.7.1 Description
As ambient illumination levels decline from day to night-time levels, the visual system
undergoes a corresponding increase in sensitivity. This process, called dark adaptation,
is mediated by two complementary retinal photoreceptor systems; together they provide
the human observer with visual sensitivity over a wide range of light intensities (about
12-log units). The cone-based photopic system, which operates at higher light
(luminance levels in the range of 10
0
to 10
7
cd/m
2
), provides the observer with fine
spatial resolution (i.e., acuity) and color vision. This system reaches its maximum
sensitivity within the first 8 to 10 minutes of dark adaptation. The highly sensitive but
colour-blind rod-based scotopic system mediates perception at low light levels from
about 10
-6
to 10
1
cd/m
2
. It may not reach its maximum sensitivity for 30 to 40 minutes
into the dark adaptation process.
The transition from the photopic to scotopic system accounts for the loss of acuity and
colour vision that occurs with falling light levels. The relatively narrow 1-log unit
luminance range over which both systems are functional (from 10
0
to 10
1
cd/m
2
) is called
mesopic vision. Essentially then, there are two levels of night vision (e.g., Hovis, 2000).
In one (mesopic), there is sufficient light for the cones to provide some colour vision and
enhance acuity (e.g., distant objects illuminated at night by automobile headlights). In
Vision Standards for Aircrew
31
the other (scotopic), the conditions are so dark (e.g., a dark overcast night) that only rods
operate, acuity is very poor and no colour is seen. For some observers, very low-light
levels can also trigger a process known as a dark shift, in which the eye’s accommodation
level increases. The resultant shift of focus to within a meter or two of the observer
causes distant objects to be blurred in a phenomenon known as night myopia.
With age, there a progressive loss of both the photopic and scotopic sensitivity (e.g.,
Kline & Scialfa, 1996); however, the scotopic decline is considerably more pronounced
(Jackson & Owsley, 2000). As a result, driving and everyday visual tasks become
steadily more difficult for older observers to carry out in dim light (e.g., Kline, Kline,
Fozard, Kosnik, Schieber, & Sekuler, 1992).
3.7.2 Literature Review
Although dark adaptation is critical for tasks that must be carried out in low light, its
measurement is difficult, time-consuming and requires specialized facilities and highly-
trained personnel (e.g., Casson, 1995; National Research Council, 1985). These factors
may account for the absence of a night vision standard for pilot entry for most ASCC
countries, including Canada (ASCC, 2003). That does not mean, however, that nighttime
tests could not make an important contribution to pilot vision screening.
When Fowlkes, Kennedy, Hettinger and Harm (1993) studied the relationship between
the dark focus of accommodation and simulator sickness among young observers, they
found that those who became ill were more likely to demonstrate a tendency toward night
myopia. The authors hypothesized that the relationship might be mediated by common
parasympathetic activity. Noting that night vision goggles, virtual environments and
head-up displays all produce similar visual symptoms, they suggested that changes in
dark focus should be measured in these settings as well.
Not surprisingly, some night vision tests appear to be much more effective than
conventional daytime measures for predicting pilot performance in mesopic and scotopic
conditions. A study that compared the utility of more than 20 night vision tests
(Glovinsky, Belkin & Hammer, 1992), found three that were predictive of an observer’s
ability to detect military targets at night: dark adaptation rate (DAR), scotopic retinal
threshold (SRT) and mesopic contrast sensitivity (CS). Follow-up research to evaluate
the reliability of these tests for young observers over a two- and six-week period (Levy &
Glovinsky, 1997) found acceptable measurement stability for SRT and CS, but not the
DAR measure. The investigators concluded that the assessment of the night vision of
pilots and military personnel could be based on scotopic sensitivity after 30 minutes of
dark adaptation and contrast sensitivity (1.5, 3.0, 6.0, and 12.0 c/deg) under mesopic
illumination.
By significantly enhancing nighttime acuity and contrast sensitivity (e.g., Rabin, 1993),
night vision goggles (NVGs) allow pilots to operate far more effectively in low-light
conditions. NVGs, however, are not without problems. Numerous human factors
concerns are associated with their use (Manton, 2000). For example, light adaptation to
intensified images can handicap pilots who set cockpit luminance too low (Howard,
Vision Standards for Aircrew
32
Riegler, & Martin, 2001). The latter investigators found that a luminance difference of
2.0 or more log units increased response time to cockpit instrumentation by up to 5.5
seconds among pilots in their 20’s and by 8 to 15 seconds for older pilots.
In multi-crew cockpits, problems of luminance adaptation are occasionally mitigated with
the flying pilot using NVGs and the co-pilot monitoring the flight information displays
(Task & Griffin, 1982). NVGs may also present difficulties to pilots using optical
correction. Spectacle wearers tend to have worse acuity than non-spectacle wearers
through NVGs (Silberman, Apsey, Ivan, & Jackson, 1994) and pilots who need bifocals
may need to use a larger near-add segment to achieve decent close vision (Farr, 1989).
Finally, many older pilots may need an optical correction that exceeds the corrective
limits of the NVG (Stone, Sanders, Glick, Wiley & Kimball, 1980).
Problems of distance (depth) judgment (e.g., Sheehy & Wilkinson, 1989) and spatial
disorientation with NVGs can be acute, especially in rotary wing aircraft operations
(Holmes, Bunting, Brown, Hiatt, Braithwaite & Harrigan, 2003). An analysis of U.S.
Army helicopter accidents found that approximately 45% of spatial-disorientation
accidents were associated with the use of NVGs. Accident rates with them were more
than five times higher than daytime rates (Braithwaite, Douglass, Durnford, & Lucas,
1998). Research has shown that problems of spatial disorientation with NVGs can be
reduced with effective training that emphasizes adjusting the NVG to the least-minus
(myopia) correction required (Kotulak & Morse, 1994) and using a standard target and
adjustment procedure (DeVilbiss, Ercoline, & Antonio, 1994).
3.7.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of night vision. Landing approaches at night are demanding, especially
in the rain. The SMEs also indicated that twilight and late dusk lower the contrast
between objects, whether flying over water or land. One example they provided was the
ability to detect a smaller hill directly in front of a larger hill when flying towards the
hills. In terms of night vision, there may not be enough distinguishing factors between
the larger and smaller hill to determine that there are actually two distinct hills in the
flight path. This is especially dangerous for a low-flying fixed wing plane (in
comparison to a helicopter), as the pilots of these aircraft have less time to react and
fewer options for action. The SMEs also noted that it is not uncommon to have to adjust
(adapt) rapidly to dim lighting after flying in bright conditions.
Night vision was also important for one SME who recalled a time when he was trying to
identify a submarine at night. Considering a submarine at night may look very similar to
a sailboat, misidentifications do occur. As a result, sailboats have occasionally had
sonobuoys dropped near or on them, due to pilots incorrectly identifying them as
submarines.
The SMEs also reported the challenges associated with finding airport runway lights
amongst surrounding bright city lights at night. They indicated that it is a difficult task to
distinguish the runway lights within the myriad of additional city lights that may also be
Vision Standards for Aircrew
33
brighter than the runway lights. To mitigate this difficulty, the SMEs reported they look
for patterns in city lights in order to identify an airport runway strip. Complicating this
task though, is the fact that any one of the lights presumed to be a city light may actually
be another aircraft flying directly in the flightpath of a pilot attempting to land their
aircraft.
Several of the SMEs noted the visual difficulties associated with NVGs. Among them,
they need to be focused carefully before use, the focus adjustment range is limited (2.0
D), the visual field is limited (reported to be 40 deg), the green monochrome display
leaves colour after-effects (referred to as “pink eye”) and it is very difficult to judge
depth through the NVGs. The pilots also indicated that it is difficult to keep the cockpit
in the field of view through NVGs, in order to maintain a spatial reference.
3.8 Depth Perception
3.8.1 Description
On dynamic tasks such as walking, driving a car, or flying a plane, depth information
regarding the position of objects in relation to ones’ current position (egocentric
localization), as well as the location of objects relative to one another (relative
localization), are critical. Depth judgments are based on information from each eye alone
(monocular depth information) as well as the two eyes working together (binocular depth
perception). Considerable monocular depth information can be acquired from the so-
called “pictorial cues” that can be represented on a two-dimensional surface (e.g., a photo
or painting). These include object overlap, shading and shadow, relative size, height in
the field of view, texture gradient, linear perspective and aerial or atmospheric
perspective. Monocular depth information can also be derived from dynamic cues such
as accommodation and convergence, motion parallax (faster relative motion for near
objects) and deletion and accretion (the rate at which, in passing, a close object covers or
reveals a more distant surface).
Binocular depth perception (stereopsis) is based on a comparison of how the image
information in the two eyes differs due to their lateral separation (about 63 mm centre-to-
centre for the average human observer). This perspective induced image difference,
referred to as “retinal disparity”, is the basis for “stereo” viewing systems (e.g., the
“Viewmaster”). The ability to appreciate such fine binocular perspective differences (as
low as three to five sec of arc) is termed stereoacuity. Monocular eye conditions can
impair stereoacuity and if present in early development (e.g., amblyopia, heterophoria,
heterotropia), may even preclude its development. Since vision losses are more prevalent
in the older eye, so too are failures of stereopsis (e.g., Wright & Wormald, 1992).
3.8.2 Literature Review
Good depth perception is critical to the pilot; when visual conditions are ambiguous (e.g.,
poor atmospheric conditions) or misleading regarding altitude, horizon, slope (Holmes, et
al., 2003) or when information and vestibular information is in conflict (Regan, 1995),
spatial disorientation can result. Fortunately, the redundancy of visually available
Vision Standards for Aircrew
34
distance and location information sources facilitates performance across most task
conditions. For example, although NVGs appear to induce a transient loss of
stereoscopic depth perception, depth perception based on monocular information is
unimpaired (Sheehy & Wilkinson, 1989). For most jobs and tasks, stereopsis appears to
be critical only when other sources of depth information are not available (Casson, 1995),
or when viewing conditions are poor (Jones & Lee, 1981). Few occupations have a
binocular vision requirement (Beard, Hisle, & Ahumada, 2002; Good, Weaver, &
Augsberger, 1996) and there is reason to question its relevance as a visual standard for
flying. Snyder and Lezotte (1993) found that attrition rates in the U.S. Air Force were
not different for individuals with good or poor distance stereopsis. After a review of its
theoretical and empirical bases, Diepgen (1993) concluded that there is not sufficient
reason for having a stereopsis standard for pilots. Consistent with this view, the
requirements for pilot entry to the CF identify a test that can be administered for
stereoacuity (the Titmus), however, there is no standard or threshold (ASCC, 2003).
3.8.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of depth perception. For example, interpreting the radar plot is a depth
critical task. A radar plot may display four contacts on it, all at differing altitudes and the
pilot must be able to resolve the true picture of the outside world with respect to the plot.
In essence, the pilot must be able to view a two-dimensional radar display and mentally
map this picture in three dimensions in order to establish situational awareness that is true
to reality.
The SMEs also noted the importance of depth perception when landing an aircraft
adjacent to obstacles and determining the distance to those obstacles. For example, the
helicopter pilots must be aware of the clearance afforded from the helicopter rotor blades
to surrounding objects. Another example illustrating the importance of depth perception
is the task of formation flying; the ability to distinguish the distance between aircraft is a
safety critical element of this piloting task. Further, depth judgment is more difficult in
low contrast conditions and at night. While NVGs help the pilot to see in dim conditions,
it is difficult to judge depth accurately through them and it can take a number of weeks to
learn to estimate depth perception through NVGs. The SMEs also indicated that it is
difficult to judge depth over uniform terrain and water, a problem for both landing and
low-level flight. To avoid this, some rotary-wing pilots reported that when possible they
increase altitude when flying over water.
3.9 Motion Perception
3.9.1 Description
Sensitivity to motion can be measured in a variety of ways, but none of them have been
standardized for large-scale clinical application. One laboratory approach is to determine
the minimum spatial displacement of a visual target that produces discriminable
movement, a measure known as the oscillatory motion displacement threshold (OMDT).
Investigators have also used random dot motion displays to study the minimum
Vision Standards for Aircrew
35
proportion of elements moving in a common direction that yields directional motion (i.e.,
the coherence threshold). Another measure, used in both lab and field studies, is to have
observers estimate the time at which a moving target will arrive at a specified location or
collide with another object. Field studies may also ask observers to estimate the velocity,
acceleration and/or direction of a target vehicle or object.
There is compelling laboratory evidence of age-related declines in motion sensitivity as
measured by OMDTs (Kline, Culham, Bartel & Lynk, 2001) and coherence thresholds
(Trick & Silverman, 1991). A lab study by Andersen, Cisneros, Saidpour and Atchley
(2000) had observers view displays simulating a 3-D environment with obstacles lying in
the path of target motion. As target motion decelerated at a constant rate, the obstacle
was blacked out. On some trials, the rate of deceleration would result in a “collision”
with an obstacle, on others it would not. The proportion of judgments of a collision on
no-collision trials was greater for older than younger observers. This led the authors to
suggest that the elevated accident rates of older drivers might be due in part to an
inability to detect collisions at high speeds. Deficits on such lab tasks, however, may not
characterize aging effects on real-world judgments of speed.
Scialfa, Guzy, Leibowitz, Garvey and Tyrrell (1991) examined age differences in the
magnitude estimations of velocity for automobiles traveling at speeds varying from 15-50
MPH (24-80 KPH). Young and old observers both tended to underestimate the speed of
slow-moving vehicles and overestimate the speed of rapid ones, but the effect was less
pronounced for older observers. The resulting psychophysical functions relating
perceived speed to actual speed, suggested that older observers were less sensitive to
relative changes in velocity, but that their absolute judgments of speed were more
accurate than young observers. However, the implications of these findings regarding the
effects of driver age on driving safety are unclear.
3.9.2 Literature Review
The accuracy of motion perception is affected by a range of situational variables,
including the effects of motion adaptation, misattribution of the motion, poor visibility
and the visibility of effective texture information from the terrain. When Stewart and
Clark (1975) measured the effects of CRT-presented rotary motion on airline pilots’
response speed to horizontal acceleration, reaction time increased directly with the
duration of exposure to rotary motion. Gray and Regan (2000) studied the effect of
adaptation to image expansion in a driving simulator overtaking task. After driving on a
straight roadway, drivers initiated overtaking later on, in comparison to a similar period
of driving through curved roadway, or viewing a static scene. The authors concluded that
the difference was due to misestimating headway time caused by local adaptation of the
looming detectors that signal motion in depth. They later suggested (Regan & Gray,
2001) that drivers should vary their direction of gaze during extended driving on straight
empty roads, in order to reduce local motion adaptation due to retinal image expansion
and thus reduce errors in judging time to collision. Perhaps similar advice would benefit
pilots engaged in prolonged low-level flight.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
36
Misattribution of the source of motion, (that occurs in a motion-depth illusion as based on
the “far-anchor effect”), can also erode the accuracy of motion perception. Specifically,
in limited viewing conditions, motion may be attributed to the further of two targets when
only the near one is actually moving (Young, Mershon & Hicks, 2002). In a study with
college student observers, Young et al. found that this illusion was not attenuated by
fixational instructions, feedback, or the vertical on-screen separation of the targets. They
noted the implications of problems in judging motion in depth for midair collisions and
ground-incursion incidents when visibility is reduced. Similarly, after comparing the
ability of pilots and non-pilots to make simulated night landing approaches, Mertens
(1978) suggested that the visual illusions associated with the ineffectiveness of motion
parallax in night conditions might be an important contributor to night approach
problems.
The terrain and the ability to see it, appear to contribute importantly to a pilot’s judgment
of speed, altitude and direction of motion in low-level flight and landing. Lintern and Liu
(1991) found that the implicit specification of an on-screen horizon using texture
contributed to a more accurate perception of simulated glide slope angle. Texture has
also been found to affect simulated altitude control at higher speeds (Flach, Warren,
Garness, Kelly, & Stanard, 1997). Flight over water appears to create special problems
for pilots’ perception of self-motion. Ungs (1989) surveyed 267 U.S. Coast Guard
helicopter pilots regarding the occurrence of illusory vection while flying over water in
different sea and lighting conditions. The illusion was reported by over 92% of the pilots
and almost 85% of them indicated that its likelihood was increased in dark conditions.
They were also somewhat more likely to report the problem as worse over rough (about
46%) than smooth seas (about 38%).
The absence of a required motion sensitivity threshold for pilot entry into the CF is
consistent with the lack of a clinically accepted standard for its measurement. Nor does
such a standard seem feasible currently, given the wide range of different motion tasks,
their broad susceptibility to contextual variables and high inter- (e.g., Otakeno, Matthews,
Folio, Previc & Lessard, 2002) and intra-individual variability (e.g., Hong & Regan,
1989) in sensitivity to different aspects of motion.
3.9.3 Related Tasks
The SME discussion identified common pilot tasks that are challenging to the visual
system in terms of motion perception. Consistent with Ungs (1989) findings of vision
while flying over water, the helicopter pilots reported that the task of hovering in a stable
position over a moving ship is difficult to perform. The difficulty lies in discerning how
the aircraft is moving in relation to the ship and how the ship is moving in relation to the
aircraft in order to maintain aircraft position. Motion perception is also a necessary
visual function when detecting other air traffic in the sky, as it is the relative motion of
other aircraft that first cues a pilot to their presence.
Motion perception is important within aircraft cockpits as well. Many lights in the
cockpit will flicker or flash to indicate a warning to the pilot. However, these warning
lights may or may not be in the direct field of view of the pilot. Another difficult visual
Vision Standards for Aircrew
37
task related to motion perception is the ability to detect whether or not helicopter main
blades are rotating. When helicopter blades are rotating, they may be spinning so fast
that it is difficult to discern whether they are in fact actually spinning (due to an optical
illusion effect), making this a safety critical task.
3.10 Refractive Error and Optical Correction
3.10.1 Description
Four common types of refractive problems, myopia, hyperopia, astigmatism, and
presbyopia can degrade the quality of vision. In myopia (nearsightedness), the eye’s
optic media are too strong relative to eyeball length. This causes images to be focused in
front of the retina. A negative-sphere (concave) ophthalmic lens is prescribed to move
the focal plane back to the retina and improve distance vision. In hyperopia (far-
sightedness), the image plane is beyond the retina due to insufficient refractive power
relative to eyeball length. A positive sphere (convex) corrective lens is used to add
refractive power and bring near objects to better focus.
Astigmatism refers to unevenness of image focus, and is usually due to an irregular
cornea - one shaped more like a football than a basketball. Such an eye has greater
refractive power and greater visual clarity at some orientations than others. Astigmatism
is labeled depending on the orientation at which it occurs. If refractive power is greater
in the vertical plane (i.e., 90°± 25
0
), it is termed with-the-rule; if it is closer to the
horizontal axis (0°± 25
0
), it is termed against-the-rule. Eyes with greater focusing power
between the vertical and horizontal axes are said to manifest oblique astigmatism.
Astigmatism is corrected optically using a cylindrical lens of the appropriate power and
axis.
Presbyopia is the progressive age-related loss of focusing (accommodative) power that
leads to a steady recession of the near point of vision (see section 3.2.1 regarding the
effects of presbyopia on near acuity). A positive sphere lens (e.g., reading glasses) or
lens segment (e.g., bifocals or trifocals) can be used to compensate for the older eye’s lost
focusing capacity.
Refractive errors necessitating the use of corrective lenses are increasingly prevalent with
increasing age. Wang, Klein, Klein, and Moss (1994) reported that far-sightedness
(hyperopia) in excess of .5 diopters in the right-eye rose from about 22% in those 43 to
54 years to almost 69% among those 75 and older. The prevalence of nearsightedness
(myopia) in excess of 0.5 diopters, declined from about 43% to 14%. Astigmatism is also
more prevalent in the later years and is frequently accompanied by a change in axis. For
example, Gudmundsdottir, Jonasson, Jonsson, Stefansson, Sasaki & Sasaki (2000) found
that the prevalence of right eyes requiring ± .75 D or more of cylindrical lens correction
increased markedly with age for both men and women. They observed a decline with age
in the prevalence of with-the-rule astigmatism and a marked elevation in both against-
the-rule and oblique astigmatism.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
38
3.10.2 Literature Review
The importance of corrective eyewear is highlighted by the significant proportion of
aviators in both military and civilian aviation that require visual correction. A sample of
over 2,000 members of the U.S. Air Force found that almost 20% of pilots and
approximately 50% of navigators were required to wear corrective lenses while flying
(Provines, Woessner, Rahe, & Tredici, 1983). Similar levels were found in a later study
of the records of over 5,000 active U.S. Air Force personnel (Miller, Woessner, Dennis,
O’Neal & Green, 1990); 27.4% of pilots and 51.5% of navigators/weapons systems
operators required corrective lenses. While myopia was the predominant refractive error,
clinically significant astigmatism was also common (e.g., 33% of pilots). Among pilots
who wore spectacles, more than 12% required bifocals. These findings contrast with
those at the pilots’ time of entry into the Air Force when emmetropia (no refractive error)
and hyperopia were considerably more common. Based on a study of 1400 Israeli Air
Force personnel, Froom, Biger, Erel, Davidson and Shochat (1992) concluded that the
likelihood of a pilot requiring lens correction for myopia was greater if myopia was
present in one eye at the time of entry into the Air Force.
The growing impact of age-related change in refractive error and ophthalmic lens use in
aviation has been the topic of concern in several studies. In a 25-year prospective study
of Japan Air Self Defense Personnel Force, Kikukawa, Yagura and Akamatsu (1999)
found that from age 20 to 45, the proportion of participants requiring a distant visual
correction increased from 15.8% to 37.1%. Interestingly, those with best acuity at entry
showed a lower need for corrective lenses later on. The same benefit of high initial levels
of uncorrected acuity has been recorded in the Royal Australian Air Force (RAAF).
After surveying all RAAF personnel records, Mork and Watson (1993) concluded that
the highly restrictive visual refraction standards for entry into RAAF aircrew training,
relative to the USAF, were associated with a reduced prevalence of corrective lens usage.
Further, as the mean age of the U.S. civilian pilot population increased to 39.8 years from
1971 to 1991, Nakagawara, Wood, and Montgomery (1995) found a 12% rise in the
population with a near-vision restriction.
As the age of the pilot population increases, more problems associated with presbyopia
and near-vision can be expected. Hyperopia and low amplitude of accommodation in
pilots at age 20 appear to be risk factors for early presbyopia (Spierer & Shaley, 2003).
After reviewing all the medical restriction data for civil aviators in the U.S. from 1976 to
2001, Nakagawara, Montgomery and Wood (2004) found that the increase in mean pilot
age (from 36.8 to 42.3 years) was associated with a marked elevation in near-vision
restrictions (13%), a rate more than double the increase (6%) found for distance vision.
Noting that 92% of all medical restrictions as of 2001 were vision related, the authors
noted the growing challenge in eye-care and visual correction for the aging aviatior
community.
Generally, it appears that corrective lenses (contact lenses or spectacles) can be used
safely and effectively in aviation. Several studies have shown that contact lenses provide
effective vision correction for pilots (Bachman, 1990; Bickel & Barr, 1997; Polishuk, &
Raz, 1975), even for critical and hazardous missions (Mittelman, Siegel, & Still, 1993).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
39
Eyeglasses also appear to be visually effective for many tasks in the aviation
environment. Still and Temme (1992) found that nighttime fixed-wing carrier landing
scores were as good for U.S. Navy pilots who required spectacle correction as those who
did not. Similarly, a study (Froom, Ribak, Burger & Gross, 1987) found that helicopter
pilots with corrective lenses, or even minor uncorrected decreases in acuity, were not at
increased risk for a serious accident.
However, not all studies have produced such positive findings regarding eyewear use for
aircrew tasks and safety. Although a study of medically certified Category 1 Canadian
commercial and airline pilots found that the accident rates of those with high refractive
errors (+/- 5.7 D) were within the expected normal range, they were also higher than
those having less extreme refractive errors (=/- 3.5 to +/- 5.6 D). Temme and Still (1991)
found that navy pilots without eyeglasses were able to identify an “adversary” in combat
maneuvers at a distance 20% greater than pilots who wore glasses.
Based on an analysis of the safety record data of civilian pilots, Nakagawara, Wood and
Mongomery, (2002) found that contact lens use had contributed to five accidents and one
incident. In a related study, the same authors (Nakagawara, Montgomery, & Wood,
2002) found evidence in the National Transportation Safety Board (NTSB) and Federal
Aviation Administration (FAA) databases that ophthalmic devices were a contributing
factor in 19 different mishaps. These included difficulties with lost or broken eyeglasses,
problems with sunglasses, incompatibility with breathing equipment, or inappropriate
prescriptions and contact lenses. Some, but of course not all, problems of this type can
be alleviated through regular eye exams. Finally, a study of aircraft ejection events
among U.S. Navy pilots (O’Connell & Markovits, 1995) found that all eyewear was lost
in 37 of 46 cases. In all cases of eye-wear retention, the pilot’s visor was down, the
oxygen mask in place and the helmet secured; a demonstration of the importance of
enforcement of pilot adherence to standard operating procedures for these devices.
3.10.3 Related Tasks
The SMEs noted the need for pilots, including instructors, to have a good visual
correction. Even if a pilot is flying with a co-pilot or a student pilot, it cannot be assumed
that the individual is qualified to fly the aircraft alone or respond to an emergency. One
SME noted that since NVGs cannot correct astigmatism, pilots with this problem require
an optical correction. NVGs were also reported to be incompatible with progressive
lenses making bifocals or trifocals a necessity for many older pilots. Problems with
maintaining eyewear in place with severe G forces, the use of an oxygen mask, fogging
of eyeglasses or the visor in hot conditions and contact lenses in dry environments, were
also noted.
3.11 Refractive Error and Photorefractive Surgery
3.11.1 Description
Increasingly, refractive errors are treated using photorefractive surgery, LASIK (laser-
assisted in situ keratomileusis) or PRK (photorefractive keratectomy) to permanently
Vision Standards for Aircrew
40
change the curvature of the cornea. In LASIK, a hinged flap is cut into the corneal
surface. The flap is folded back, an excimer laser is used to vaporize some of the
underlying stroma, and the flap is then smoothed back into place.
PRK is similar in that it is also performed with an excimer laser, but it is used to remove
tissue from the very front surface of the cornea. In both types of surgery, the cornea is
flattened for myopia, steepened for hyperopia, and smoothed for astigmatism. Although
LASIK patients are likely to experience less discomfort and obtain good vision more
quickly, surgeons may prefer PRK for patients with larger pupils or thin corneas. While
most patients no longer need corrective lenses to carry out everyday visual tasks after
LASIK or PRK, the optical aberrations induced by surgery can affect acuity and contrast
sensitivity, particularly in dim light (Chisholm, Evans, Harlow, & Barbur, 2003; Schlote,
Derse, Wannke, Bende, & Jean, 1999; Stern, 1999).
3.11.2 Literature Review
The development of photorefractive surgery techniques for correcting refractive errors
has spawned concerns regarding their appropriateness for aviators (Markovits, 1993).
These concerns include the structural stability of the eye as well as the effects on visual
functioning post-surgery. As a result of such concerns, in some services such as the USN
and USAF, pilots who have received photorefractive surgery are prohibited from service
in combat jets (Levy, Zadok, & Barenboim, 2003).
Goodman, Johnson, Dillon, Edelhauser & Waller (2003) found that the healed LASIK
flaps on rabbit eyes were not affected by a 9-G simulated ejection. Nor were any
negative sequelae reported after an ejection by a U.S. Navy pilot 6 months after receiving
PRK (Tanzer, Schallhorn, & Brown, 2000). Although one study (Levy, Zadok, &
Barenboim, 2003) reports the case of an Israeli Air Force pilot who has performed
numerous uneventful daytime flights in a combat aircraft, concerns regarding the impact
of photorefractive surgery on nighttime vision and safety still remain.
Schallhorn, Blanton, Kaupp, Sutphin, Gordon, Goforth & Butler (1996) found that while
susceptibility to disability glare was transient in active-duty military personnel, a
prolonged reduction in the quality of vision at night was observed in one patient. Finding
that PRK patients were much more susceptible to reductions of contrast sensitivity in
glare, Schlote, Derse, Wannke, Bende and Jean (1999) concluded that the reduction of
mesopic visual function is of special concern; particularly for those who, like pilots and
professional drivers, have need of highly effective vision in low illumination conditions.
There is also evidence, however, that low contrast acuity may be used to screen pilots for
this problem (Chisholm, Evans, Harlow, & Barbur, 2003).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
41
4 Results - Tasks
The goal of the task analysis in the SME session was to determine the essential tasks
performed by CF pilots that are common and have a critical vision component. Essential
tasks were identified and the relationship between the performance of the essential tasks
and vision was evaluated.
In order to fulfill these requirements, the following steps were performed:
1. Document the essential visual tasks performed by CF pilots that are common
across all aircraft types.
2. Determine the consequences of performing the tasks improperly and the
frequency each task is performed; and
3. Select tasks that are feasible to simulate.
This procedure was followed to ensure that the proposed final vision standard will meet
the requirements of the Canadian Human Rights Act.
4.1 Common Tasks Performed by CF Pilots
There are a wide range of tasks performed by the various CF pilots. For the current
report, the list of tasks generated in the Vision Survey of CF Aircrew document (Heikens
et al., 1999) for each type of aircraft was reviewed by the experimenters in order to focus
on the most visually demanding and safety critical tasks in the SME session. The CF
Vision Survey generated a list of the most demanding tasks in terms of vision for each of
the aircraft types including: Tactical Helicopter, Search and Rescue Rotary Wing,
Maritime Patrol Rotary Wing, Fixed Wing Transport, Maritime Patrol Fixed Wing,
Fighter, Jet Trainer and Primary Trainer Rotary Wing.
The questionnaire in Appendix A lists the most visually demanding tasks as rated by the
pilots of each aircraft type, according to the results in the CF Vision Survey. The
questionnaire was then sent to the focus group SMEs prior to the discussion, in order to
obtain additional information that may also influence pilot tasks. This provided an
initiation point for the SME discussions and enabled the pilots to add or modify tasks as
they deemed necessary.
During the SME session, it was reported that a number of the pilot tasks listed in the CF
Vision Survey, and subsequently, listed in the questionnaire, were not currently
performed. For example, the survey listed ‘Hovering’ and ‘Slinging’ as tasks that
helicopter pilots perform; however, the SMEs indicated that these tasks are no longer
conducted. Similarly, a number of tasks were omitted from the CF Vision Survey (and
subsequently the questionnaire), that all pilots will be expected to perform in the future.
This includes the use of NVGs, which will become more prevalent in the future for all
night time piloting tasks. Further, during the discussion, the SMEs indicated that the
visually demanding nature of many of their tasks is related to the requirement to
Vision Standards for Aircrew
42
constantly change visual focus between near, intermediate and far distance vision. This
requirement is fatiguing to the eyes, regardless of the task being performed.
4.1.1 Vision Task Questionnaire Results
The results of the Vision Task Questionnaire are summarized below, by aircraft type:
Tactical Helicopter: a number of the tasks listed in the questionnaire for Tactical
helicopter are no longer performed by the pilots. For example, Nap of the Earth
(NOE) Night Vision Goggle (NVG) flat terrain/rough terrain, formation night
unaided and tactical approach NVG are not currently approved tasks. Advanced
NVG limitations have been placed on NVG flying, including the requirement to
stay 50 feet or above the highest obstacle when performing a low-level NVG
task. The Tactical helicopter tasks were rated as either routine or non-routine,
requiring near, intermediate and far vision, usually performed at night and may
be performed by a newly recruited pilot (0-2 years of experience).
Search and Rescue (SAR) Rotary Wing: A number of additional tasks were
added to the SAR task list as a result of the questionnaire discussion during the
focus group. These include Open water hoist (unaided day or night), Night hoist,
and Night and day confined area operations. Overall, the SAR tasks were rated
as either routine or non-routine, requiring near, intermediate and far vision,
performed under all weather conditions and may be performed by a junior pilot
with a minimum of 2-5 years of experience.
Maritime Patrol Rotary Wing: The SMEs made changes to the tasks listed for
these aircraft pilots as well. The tasks added include Sling at sea day/night and
Water landing day/night. The tasks of approach/landing on a ship at night
unaided in rough sea were rated as emergency tasks, as the SMEs indicated these
tasks would usually be performed aided, hence, it would be an emergency if
performed unaided. Other tasks were rated as either routine or non-routine, all
requiring near, intermediate and far vision, performed under all weather
conditions and may be performed by either a new recruit or a junior pilot.
Fixed Wing Transport: The SMEs also added a number of tasks to the Fixed
Wing list including tactical departure shallow/steep altitude, low level mountain
flying and tactical arrivals from low/medium and high altitudes. It was reported
that Low altitude parachute extraction NVG is a task no longer performed by
pilots. The tasks were rated as either routine or non-routine, all requiring near,
intermediate and far vision, performed under all weather conditions and
performed by a junior pilot as a minimum.
Maritime Patrol Fixed Wing (MPFW): The NVG tasks that were listed in the
questionnaire are not performed by MPFW pilots, as reported in the SME
session. Many of the tasks discussed that are performed by these pilots include a
variation of either target or smoke detection in day/night conditions, calm/rough
sea or forested terrain, and/or unaided. Many of the tasks were rated as non-
routine, all requiring near, intermediate and far vision, performed under all
weather conditions and performed by a junior pilot as a minimum.
Fighter: The SME discussion indicated that target detection is also a common
task performed by fighter pilots, however, this task must be performed while
viewing through a Head up Display (HUD). The task may also be performed at
Vision Standards for Aircrew
43
night and unaided. The task of low level flying is avoided in fighter aircraft and
if this type of task is performed, it would be considered an emergency. Fighter
tasks require near, intermediate and far vision, they may be performed under all
weather conditions and may be performed by a new recruit (excluding the
emergency task which would be performed by an intermediate pilot with five to
ten years of experience).
The questionnaire was not filled in by the SMEs for the Jet Trainer or the
Primary Trainer Rotary Wing. However, these trainers were discussed at the
SME session. The original scope of this project was to focus on conditions that
pilots would be exposed to in training (relating to the entrance/recruitment vision
standard for aircrew). However, the SME session revealed that new recruits are
not exposed to some of the more challenging flight situations experienced by
more senior pilots, thereby a vision standard based upon an entry-level task
would not encompass the most difficult tasks later performed by pilots. Some of
these tasks include NVG flying, night formation, hovering over a ship at night,
and performance in severe environmental conditions.
4.1.2 SME Discussion
From the SME discussion and the vision questionnaire results, it was determined that a
number of commonalities exist between the tasks performed in each aircraft type and that
safety critical tasks have many similar vision demands as commonly performed, routine
tasks.
It was concluded that the visual demands for each pilot task (regardless of aircraft type)
exist on a continuum between high and low visual demands, depending upon a number of
extraneous factors. These extraneous factors include the interior and exterior lighting
conditions (good/poor), a vibrating cockpit, wearing laser eye protection or NVGs, a
variety of distances challenging the visual system (near, intermediate, far and constantly
transitioning between these three distances) and a variety of environmental conditions
(night, snow, rain, dusk, dawn, fog, etc). Each of these factors may work alone or in
combination to affect the visual demands of any pilot task. The following is a list
generated from the SME discussion of tasks performed by all pilots regardless of airplane
type.
Reading VFR maps, contour maps, and approach plates
Interaction with and comprehension of CRT instrumentation, the weather radar
display and the flight management system
Target/object detection and identification
Aircraft landing and take-off
Perceiving and responding to cockpit warning lights
Detecting other aircraft and/or birds in the peripheral field of view
Determining attitude (pitch, roll and yaw) of the aircraft
Flying directly into the sun or flying with the sun directly behind the aircraft
Distinguishing differences between colour coded items, both inside and outside
of the cockpit
Determining clearance distance between the aircraft and surrounding objects
Vision Standards for Aircrew
44
Movement detection from within the aircraft cockpit (e.g., flashing warning
lights)
Night instrument approach landing
Continuous transitioning between near and far viewing (cockpit instrumentation
to exterior environment)
Low-level flying over various terrain
Fast visual accommodation from bright light to low light and vice versa
Reading emergency checklists while flying and responding appropriately
Emergency Autorotation training (although only for rotary wing pilots)
These tasks may be influenced by any one of the extraneous factors previously listed
above.
4.2 Consequences of Improper Performance
With each of these tasks, there are consequences related to improper performance ranging
from mild (mistake determined and correction performed) to severe (accident resulting in
death or destruction of property). The SMEs indicated that all of the common tasks listed
above are associated with flying, and therefore, if performed improperly can lead to
severe consequences.
4.3 Task Scenarios
The SME discussion enabled the experimenters to ascertain visual acuity demand
commonalities across all aircraft types. This is important for the purpose of the visual
acuity test in order to establish a bona fide entrance standard that may be applicable to all
CF pilot recruits, regardless of the type of aircraft eventually flown. Considering visual
acuity can be further decomposed into near and far acuity, both of these parameters
should be simulated and tested to establish the entrance standard. Again, the scenarios
must involve visual acuity functions that are applicable to all aircraft.
4.3.1 Near Visual Acuity Task
One common and critical near visual acuity task that all CF pilots must perform
(regardless of aircraft type) is reading and understanding approach plates in order to land
an aircraft at night. Again, this task is common, yet visually demanding and correct
performance is critical. The SMEs discussed extraneous factors that may be present to
make this task more difficult including:
Poor interior lighting conditions: For example, the Sea King has red interior
cockpit lighting and the interior lights used to view approach plates at night are
generally very dim in most cockpits;
Poor environmental visibility: This task must be performed in any kind of night
time environmental conditions including bright sunset, snowstorm, thunderstorm,
thick cloud cover, etc. The SMEs indicated that this task is especially difficult at
night because of the use of cockpit lighting which inhibits night vision adaptation
and renders visibility outside of the cockpit extremely difficult;
Vision Standards for Aircrew
45
Vibrating cockpit: This task is made further challenging by the vibration of the
cockpit. Again, the amount of vibration will be dependant upon the weather
conditions experienced. For example, high winds will contribute to a vibrating
cockpit which will make this focused reading task extremely difficult;
Vision Enhancers: Pilots may be expected to perform this task while wearing
either laser eye protection or NVGs. Again, this increases the task difficulty, as
the approach plates are colour coded and NVGs inhibit colour visibility and
distinction. NVGs also inhibit the visible FOV available to the pilot and make the
transition from near, intermediate and far vision very difficult (pilots have to look
down below the NVG to see near objects);
Vision Transition: Pilots must be able to look at their cockpit instrumentation
(near and colour vision), to their immediate surroundings (intermediate vision), to
distant objects (far vision) for detection and identification purposes. The constant
transition between these distances is a challenge to the visual system;
Combination: Reading and understanding approach plates is a commonly
performed pilot task across all aircraft. The combination of any or all of the
above listed extraneous factors can challenge the visual system and may render
the task of approach plate reading extremely difficult. Any of these extraneous
factors may work alone or in combination to challenge the visual system of any
pilot, in any type of aircraft, at any time.
As a result, it was decided that the task of reading and understanding approach plates in
order to land an aircraft at night was highly suitable for testing the near visual acuity
entry requirement of pilots.
4.3.2 Far Visual Acuity Task
One common and critical far acuity task that all CF pilots must perform frequently
(regardless of aircraft type) is target identification. Again, this task is common, yet
visually demanding and correct performance is critical. The number and type of targets
that pilots must identify are numerous, including ship identification lettering, search and
rescue for people stranded in the sea, birds in the sky, other aircraft in the sky, ground
target vehicles, crash sites and runway lights. One far acuity (target identification) task
that is common across all aircraft is visual identification of targets associated with aircraft
landing and approach for landing. This task is common for all aircraft types, although
rotary wing pilots may have more reaction time (longer approach time, same angle). This
task will involve air to ground reconnaissance of the landing area. There is also an
element of criticality associated with this task, due to the limited time to make
decisions/act before the situation becomes a potential emergency. Further, if the pilot
misses the runway, the procedures for a ‘missed runway’ will have to be followed (which
includes reading the approach plates and determining the emergency procedure when a
runway has been overshot). The SMEs discussed extraneous factors that may be present
to make this task more difficult including:
Poor environmental visibility: This task must be performed in any kind of
environmental conditions including bright sunshine, snowstorm, thunderstorm,
thick cloud cover, night etc;
Vision Standards for Aircrew
46
Vibrating cockpit: This task is made further challenging by the vibration of the
cockpit. Again, the amount of vibration will be dependant upon the weather
conditions experienced. For example, high winds will contribute to a vibrating
cockpit which will make a target identification task extremely difficult;
Vision Enhancers: Pilots may be expected to perform this task while wearing
either laser eye protection or NVGs. Again, this increases the task difficulty, as
NVGs inhibit colour visibility and distinction. NVGs also inhibit the visible FOV
available to the pilot and increase the difficulty in transitioning between near,
intermediate and far vision (pilots have to look down below the NVG to see near
objects);
Vision Transition: Pilots must be able to look at their cockpit instrumentation
(near and colour vision), to their immediate surroundings (intermediate vision), to
distant objects (far vision) during a target identification task. The constant
transition between these distances is also challenging to the visual system;
Combination: The combination of all of the above listed extraneous factors can
challenge the visual system and render the task of target identification during
landing/approach extremely difficult. Any of these extraneous factors may work
alone or in combination to challenge the visual system of any pilot, in any type of
aircraft, at any time.
As a result, it was decided that the task of target identification during landing/approach
was highly suitable for testing the far visual acuity entrance requirement of pilots.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
47
5 Results - Proposed Test Scenarios
Based on results of the literature review, questionnaire and SME discussion, a high-level
experimental plan for testing the selected near and far visual acuity tasks has been
developed. This plan involves the conduct of two experimental scenarios designed to
obtain objective evidence of the relationship between visual acuity level and task
performance.
As for any visually demanding occupation, it is axiomatic that vision standards for
aircrew should be based on a comprehensive evaluation of all the visual demands of
operational tasks across the full range of viewing conditions under which they are
actually carried out. The absence of the resources and time required to advance such an
ideal research model, however, mandate the use of more cost-effective simulation
approaches that are based on careful analyses of the most visually demanding tasks and
conditions that occur operationally.
The level of resources available to support the research, in combination with client
priorities, will determine which one of three possible levels of fidelity to operating
conditions is selected: 1. Real world tasks, 2. High-fidelity simulation using CF flight
simulators, or 3. Low-fidelity simulation conducted in a laboratory. For both the high-
and low-fidelity task simulations, a systematic effort will be made to ensure that the
relevant task variables are representative of actual operational conditions (e.g., contrast
levels, size scale, etc.).
High- and low- fidelity flight simulators are proposed in order to allow standardization of
lighting levels, environmental conditions and to control the size and distance of test
items. This will also allow control of luminance and contrast levels so that any
differences between the testing time of day will not likely have a confounding effect due
to the randomization of the conditions.
Of the simulation options available, high-fidelity simulation using CF flight simulators
would be preferred and in fact, may be feasible for evaluating the near-acuity demands of
extracting critical information from landing approach plates (see description of Near
Visual Acuity Test Scenario below) under realistic cockpit conditions (e.g., lighting,
environmental complexity, competing task demands, etc.). Considering the complexity,
costs and perhaps limited availability of CF flight simulators, however, the Far Visual
Acuity Test Scenario (described below) may need to be conducted using a lab-based low-
fidelity simulation. It is critical that such an effort be initiated with an understanding of
the costs, benefits and anticipated limitations of such simulation research.
Investigation into realistic environmental conditions will be conducted to determine if
experimentation can be conducted in field conditions. However, consideration must be
given to the tradeoff between conducting task simulations in realistic environments and
having complete control over the experimental environment.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
48
5.1 Lab-Based Simulation
The advantages of a Lab-Based Simulation are listed below:
1.) Fast, cost-effective “scenario” development.
2.) Testing in a safe, highly stable, constantly accessible environment.
3.) Precise control over viewing conditions, relevant target and background visual
parameters (e.g., stimulus size, luminance, contrast, colour, location, rate of motion),
and observer variables (e.g., visual health, light adaptation state, spatial vision, colour
vision).
4.) The capacity to relate observer visual characteristics (e.g., gaze direction, optical
correction, visual field, acuity, contrast sensitivity) precisely to on-screen display
elements (size, colour, field location).
The limitations of a Lab-Based Simulation Approach include the following:
1.) Testing with computer displays does not necessitate multiple-task integration
characteristic of simulator and real-world flying tasks.
2.) The “safe” lab environment is not amenable to the study of realistic pilot decision-
making and risk-taking.
3.) Real-world weather conditions (rain, fog) as well as many adverse viewing conditions
cannot be represented adequately.
4.) True 3-D (i.e., binocular or stereopsis) depth information is not available in
conventional monitor displays; they provide only monocular pictorial and relative
motion monocular cues to depth.
To maximize the generalizability of lab findings to operational tasks and conditions, a
systematic evaluation of the pilot visual tasks and the range of field conditions under
which they are likely to be carried out should be conducted. This will include detailed
task analyses to supplement the information provided by the review of the research
literature and the many insights provided by the SMEs. Once the critical task demands
have been determined, the stimulus dimensions (e.g., size, luminance, contrast, colour)
and environmental variables (e.g., lighting, glare, competing tasks, task allocation in
single-pilot vs. multi-crew cockpits) likely to be encountered operationally will be
measured. To facilitate the subsequent development of the specific experimental plan,
another SME session is recommended to verify the simulation scenario(s) “realism”,
likelihood and scale.
5.2 Near Visual Acuity Test Scenario
All aircrew regardless of aircraft type of or mission, depend critically on the information
provided by approach plates in landing. Thus, measures of the ability to extract
information from them as a function of varied acuity and lighting is proposed for the near
task. Although this study could be conducted in the lab using lighting levels similar to
those in a cockpit, testing in a CF flight simulator would provide a more realistic
environment for this (in fact, given differences in rotary and fixed-wing operations and
the wide variation in cockpit lighting levels and spectral characteristics, it would be
preferable to conduct the near acuity scenario in more than one CF flight simulator).
Vision Standards for Aircrew
49
5.2.1 Participants
To address the growing concern regarding age-related differences on visual function and
aircrew task performance, two groups of 12 trained CF pilots will participate in the study:
a young group aged 20 to 35 years, and an older group aged 35 to 49 years. (If there are
not sufficient numbers of pilots to complete the two age groups, as wide an age range as
possible will be recruited to allow regression analyses on pilot age.) Participants will be
screened for general and visual health as well as presenting optical prescription (if any).
Optimal near (40 cm) and far (6 m) ophthalmic correction and acuity will then be
determined for comparison of the effects of optically-degraded acuity with the best-
corrected levels. Pilots with levels of refractive error acceptable to the CF will be
included, but will have no prior history of eye surgery.
5.2.2 Method
A vision questionnaire will first be administered to the participants to determine their use
of glasses and/or contact lenses while on duty and any visual problems that they have
encountered in flying. They will then be tested for both high- and low-contrast (Small
Letter Contrast Test) near acuity (40 cm) and then refracted to best near acuity. The
levels of positive sphere blur required to induce 6/9, 6/12 and 6/18 near acuity will then
be determined
Participants’ ability (accuracy and latency) to find pre-cued critical target details of 3
sizes (small, medium and large-bold) on the approach plates will be determined at 40 cm
for Best, 6/9, 6/12 and 6/18 near acuity levels for daylight, dusk/dawn and night cockpit
lighting levels. The testing order will be counterbalanced.
5.2.3 Results
Response accuracy and latency data will be analyzed initially using Age (2) X Acuity (4)
X Lighting (3) X Plate Detail (3) split-plot ANOVAs. The “predictive” relationships
between optical correction, visual problems while flying, high-contrast acuity and low-
contrast acuity will be determined across and within age groups for the different lighting
conditions using correlation techniques.
5.3 Far Visual Acuity Test Scenario
To be representative of the visual demands faced by pilots on different missions and
flying different aircraft types, the far task will examine the ability to detect and identify
surface objects (e.g., runway hazards, obstacles to ground landing or SAR over water) in
daylight and dusk/dawn lighting as a function of participant acuity. Although testing in a
CF flight simulator would be more realistic and thus preferable, a lab-based simulation
may be the only feasible alternative. The method proposed below is based on this
assumption.
5.3.1 Participants
Except for testing high- and low-contrast acuity at 2 m, rather than 40 cm, the
participants, their background and visual screening will be the same as described in the
Near Visual Acuity scenario above.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
50
5.3.2 Method
5.3.2.1 Apparatus and Materials
Target stimuli will be five realistic objects of similar size, varied across three levels of
luminance contrast relative to the background scene in which they appear. They will be
presented at 2 m on a high-resolution cinema (30-in) display under the control of an
Apple G5 dual-processor computer. The target stimuli will appear on realistic digitally
photographed background day and dusk/dawn scenes (e.g., runways, grass, water, etc.).
5.3.2.2 Procedure
To simulate a landing approach, scene size will be increased in small discrete steps
(although a continuous dynamic looming scene would be more realistic, it would
confound the effects of response speed and visual acuity and also make separate
measures of target detection and identification impossible.). The participants’ task will
be to determine if any of the five pre-cued target hazards is present (target-present trials)
in the defined “landing area”, or not (target-absent trials) and then, as scene size increases
further, to identify it. The size at which the presence (i.e., location) or absence of a target
is correctly detected as well as the size at which it is correctly identified will both be
recorded. Hazard detection and identification will be determined for four acuity levels at
the 2-m test distance: Best, 6/9, 6/12, 6/18 and 6/24 in simulated day and dusk/dawn
conditions. The testing order will be counterbalanced.
5.3.3 Results
Detection and identification thresholds data will be analyzed separately using an Age (2)
X Acuity (5) X Lighting (2) X Target Contrast (3) split-plot ANOVAs. Correlation
techniques will be used to establish the relationships between optical correction, visual
problems while flying, high-contrast acuity and low-contrast acuity (across and within
age groups) under the different lighting conditions for both target detection and
identification and also the relationships between the tow measures in the different
conditions.
5.4 General Experimental Plan
The general experimental plan is described in the sections below.
5.4.1 Clarification of Tasks
To facilitate the development of a detailed experimental plan, another SME session is
recommended to:
Verify the realism of the simulation scenarios,
Subdivide the scenarios into component tasks,
Validate the task list,
Prioritize the pilot tasks in terms of mission criticality and pilot safety,
Review and characterize the visual demands of critical pilot tasks, and
Review emergency pilot procedures and critical incidents.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
51
As stated previously, a vision questionnaire will also be administered to gather
information related to the use of glasses and/or contact lenses while on duty. The
purpose of determining the use of glasses and contact lens use is to acquire information
regarding the current practices of pilots with less than 6/6 vision.
5.4.2 Randomizing Conditions
Experiments will be within-subjects randomized designs. The primary independent
variables will be the levels of simulated visual acuity. The data gathered will allow a
determination of the utility of the current CF high-contrast acuity standard for assessing
near and far performance under varied “real-world” lighting levels. The proposed
experimental plan will also allow a comparison of the predictive value of high- and low-
contrast acuity tests in different lighting conditions.
Conditions will be randomized across trials so that a number of participants will be tested
with the best visual acuity condition on the first trial while others will be tested with the
worst visual acuity condition on the first trial. Care will be taken to ensure the test order
of the conditions and the presence and location of specific target items will be evenly
distributed across participants in order to minimize learning effects.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
52
6 Discussion
Vision standards are critical to the effective and safe conduct of many tasks and this is
particularly evident for many of the tasks involved in flight operations. Good vision is a
vital requirement for mission success in many piloting tasks. As indicated in other
sections of this report, there are a number of visual functions that may be considered in
determining an appropriate vision standard. The original scope of this study focused on
visual acuity, both near and far, and development of an uncorrected visual acuity standard
for CF pilots. These aspects have been investigated with respect to developing a vision
standard, while also giving consideration to other visual functions, and the adoption of a
corrected vision standard.
6.1 Visual Acuity
Review of the literature revealed no studies or evaluations that supported or explained the
origin of any of the present CF Aircrew visual acuity standards. Many vision standards
appear to be based on expert opinion rather than job task analysis and empirical testing.
Vision standards should be based on a demonstration that a certain level of acuity is
actually needed to perform essential tasks safely and effectively.
6.1.1 Near Acuity Vision Standard
The proposed experimental task for near acuity is the ability to extract information from
approach plates as a function of varied acuity and lighting. All aircrew, regardless of
aircraft type or mission, depend critically on the information provided by approach plates
in landing. No literature is available to substantiate the borrowing of near acuity vision
standards from other occupations or countries. However, a few papers indicate that a
near acuity occupational vision standard for pilots might be empirically derived. As
described in the literature review, two empirical studies experimentally evaluated the
minimal uncorrected visual acuity requirement for military aircrew and air control
personnel. Mann and Hovis. (1996) highlighted that a major decrement in pilot
performance during an IFR approach was the inability to read approach charts, maps and
radio settings at acuity below .50 logMar (about 3 minarc or 16/50). This finding lends
support to the selection of reading approach plates during landing as a critical pilot task
and demonstrated performance degradation related to optical blur. However, these
measures are not sufficient to set acceptable levels of near visual acuity because they
were not intended to empirically justify a vision standard for military pilots. The
proposed experimental plan will extend the previous research to investigate the effect of
varying the size of information on the approach plates, as well as integration of high- and
low-contrast conditions.
6.1.2 Far Acuity Vision Standard
The proposed experimental task for far acuity is the ability to detect and identify surface
objects (e.g., runway hazards, obstacles to ground landing, or SAR over water) in
daylight and dusk/dawn lighting as a function of participant acuity. No literature is
available to substantiate the borrowing of vision standards from other occupations or
countries. However, as described in the literature review, there is some indication that
Vision Standards for Aircrew
53
for many occupations, any degradation of visual acuity below 20/20 is associated with
degraded performance in resolving fine detail at distance. Even small reductions in acuity
have been shown to impair performance on detection tasks. It is unclear if tasks from
other occupations suitably overlap with those of a CF pilot to substantiate a far acuity
vision standard. Given that pilot-related detection tasks are conducted from a unique
perspective (e.g. from air-to-ground rather than ground-to-ground) and may have
different temporal considerations given a shorter exposure time (related to a high travel
speed of aircraft), it would be unacceptable to borrow vision standards from other
occupations.
The experimental approach proposed in this study is intended to extend the research by
better defining the relationship between visual acuity and the distances required for safe
and effective hazard detection and identification. Detection tasks and identification tasks
will be investigated separately. Detection tasks involve many visual functions whereas
identification relies more heavily on visual acuity. Separation of detection and
identification tasks will more clearly characterize the visual acuity required to identify
items at different distances. Characterizing the relationship between distance and visual
acuity for the selected pilot related task will facilitate a determination of the necessary
visual acuity given real-world distances experienced by CF pilots for target identification.
6.2 Potential Visual Functions to Consider in a Vision Standard
While the focus of this study was on visual acuity, using visual acuity as the sole measure
to test vision may be too restrictive. Other visual functions may be useful in developing a
standard that is more comprehensive by considering other visual parameters that affect
task performance. Studies with pilots conducted in simulators and in field trials have
shown that acuity alone is not an absolute indicator of actual task performance (Ginsburg
et al., 1982; Ginsburg, et al., 1983). Regardless of these findings, visual acuity is still
used as the primary indicator as to whether or not a person can see well enough to drive
or pilot vehicles safely.
Other components of visual function, their associated diagnostic tests, and levels
appropriate to flight operations were investigated in this study. Visual functions
investigated included contrast sensitivity, visual fields, glare sensitivity, colour vision,
night vision, depth perception, and motion perception. The literature reviews suggest that
other visual functions may be as good or better discriminators of visual capability than
visual acuity. For example, contrast sensitivity is not traditionally included in
occupational vision standards, yet it has been found to be a better predictor of target
detection and recognition than standard visual acuity measures for pilot’s attempting to
detect ground-to-air targets in field studies (Ginsburg et al., 1983) and in simulators
(Ginsburg et al., 1982).
Although there are many potential measures of visual function, there is no evidence at
this time to suggest that any visual function is more valid than visual acuity for assessing
pilot task performance. The recommended experimental protocols of this study include
manipulation of contrast and lighting to provide additional insight into other visual
functions that warrant consideration in developing a vision standard for CF pilots.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
54
6.3 Adoption of a Corrected Visual Acuity Standard
Consideration should be given to adopt a corrected visual acuity standard. CF policy is
to recruit candidates based upon an uncorrected visual acuity standard, yet many
intermediate and senior level pilots currently rely upon vision correction. Because there
are few difficulties in correcting to a high visual acuity, adopting a corrected visual acuity
standard would facilitate a greater pool of candidates. In addition, most aviation
governing bodies, such as the FAA, no longer require a specific uncorrected visual acuity
(Beard et al., 2002). Refractive errors necessitating the use of corrective lenses are
increasingly prevalent with increasing age. Because the Canadian Forces is recruiting
older, more experienced pilots, a visual testing protocol designed to predict professional
aircrew task performance must recognize that a visual measure may be of little relevance
for younger eyes, yet may be critical in discerning problems in older eyes. Age-related
visual acuity test measures should also consider the extent to which a measure will be
valid for its requisite “predictive term” (i.e., time to next mandated test). A re-test
interval that is appropriate for young observers may be too protracted for a faster
changing older eye. The likelihood of age-related changes in refractive error, an older
recruiting age, and the probable requirement for visual correction with age, highlight a
need to consider the recognition of a corrected visual acuity standard.
Aspects of refractive error and photorefractive surgery were also investigated because
consideration of candidates with corrective eye surgery into the CF pilot occupation
would have a bearing on the visual acuity standard. Currently, personnel with corrective
eye surgery are not eligible for entry into the CF pilot occupation. Concerns include the
structural stability of the eye as well as the effects on visual functioning post-surgery.
Several studies have shown that corrective lenses can be used safely and effectively in
aviation. SME discussions indicated that pilots requiring vision correction can carry
spare glasses or they may fly with another pilot who can take over if the need arises (with
the exception of fighter pilots). However, there is some evidence that corrected lenses
contribute to a higher incidence of accidents, reduce identification capability in combat
maneuvers, and contribute to aviation mishaps. SMEs did recall incidents that occurred
as a result of wearing glasses: pilots experienced sweat dripping down their glasses, or
their glasses became foggy. It is unclear whether these problems are significant enough
to warrant an uncorrected vision standard.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
55
7 Conclusion and Recommendations
In order to perform the simulation experiments proposed in this report, a number of
action items must be performed. These include, but are not limited to:
Determine a suitable simulation (or real-world) test bed for performing the
experiments. This requires a review of the capabilities of the current flight
simulation trainers and research platforms available in the Canadian Forces.
Low-fidelity simulations, including laboratory based systems, and real-world test
beds will also be investigated. Considerations will be given to cost, schedule and
quality constraints.
Measurement of stimulus dimension and environmental variables likely to be
encountered operationally.
Determine the feasibility and timeframe associated with developing the simulator
software and hardware changes that will be required to perform the experiments.
Verify the tasks and associated visual metrics with SMEs.
Develop an experimental protocol for evaluating the simulation tests and obtain
Human Ethics Committee approval. This includes identification of additional
experimental plan details, development of an experimental introductory form,
development of a participant information sheet, and development of a vision
assessment form.
Determine the suitable population for participating in the experiments based upon
age, experience, gender, aircraft type flown, etc.
Conduct, administer and validate the visual function tests required for a sample of
aircrew in order to ascertain their acceptance for participating in the experiments.
Revise and extend this report to include a description of the above points, the
experimental design, the results and a recommendation for a vision standard for
CF aircrew.
Present the findings of the near and far visual acuity experiments.
These items will be conducted as part of the project contract extension.
The systematic and comprehensive approach proposed in this report will help to ensure
that the Canadian Human Rights Commission finds the resulting vision standard for
aircrew to be both reasonable and acceptable. The standard will ensure competent and
safe performance of tasks required by CF pilots and it will be fair, by not unnecessarily
excluding qualified candidates.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
56
8 Appendix A- Vision Task Questionnaire
See attached questionnaire.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
63
9 Appendix B – Acronyms
ARM Age-Related Maculopathy
ASCC Air Standardization Coordination Committee
BAT Brightness Acuity Tester
BCVA Best Corrected Visual Acuity
BFOR Bona Fide Occupational Requirements
BVA Best Visual Acuity
CF Canadian Forces
CS Contrast Sensitivity
CSF Contrast Sensitivity Function
DAR Dark Adaptation Rate
DRDC Defence Research and Development Canada
FAA Federal Aviation Administration
FACT Functional Acuity Contrast Test
FALANT Farnsworth Lantern Test
FOV Field of Vision
G&A Greenley & Associates
HUD Head up Display
IFR Instrument-Flight-Rules
LASIK Laser-Assisted in situ Keratomileusis
MPFW Maritime Patrol Fixed Wing
NOE Nap of the Earth
NTSB National Transportation Safety Board
NVG Night Vision Goggles
OMDT Oscillatory Motion Displacement Threshold
PIP Pseudoisochromatic Plate
PRK Photorefractive Keratectomy
RAAF Royal Australian Air Force
SAR Search and Rescue
SLCT Small Letter Contrast Test
SME Subject Matter Expert
SRT Scotopic Retinal Threshold
UCVA Uncorrected Visual Acuity
UFOV Useful Field of View
USA United States Army
USAF United States Air Force
USMC United States Marine Corps
USN United States Navy
VCTS Vistech Contrast Testing System
VF Visual Field
VFR Visual Flight Rules
Vision Standards for Aircrew
64
10 Appendix C – References
Air Standardization Coordinating Committee (ASCC). (2003). Pilot Entry Vision
Standards. Report no. 61/115/11D. Air Standardization Coordinating Committee.
Andersen, G. J., Cisneros, J., Saidpour, A., & Atchley, P. (2000). Age-related differences
in collision detection during deceleration. Psychology & Aging, 15(2), 241-252.
Anderson, S. J., & Holliday, I. E. (1995). Night driving: effects of glare from vehicle
headlights on motion perception. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 15(6), 545-
551.
Attebo, K., Mitchell, P., & Smith, W. (1996). Visual acuity and the causes of visual loss
in Australia. The Blue Mountains Eye Study. Ophthalmology, 103(3), 357-364.
Bachman, W. G. (1990). Evaluation of extended wear soft and rigid contact lens use by
Army aviators. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 61(3), 203-210.
Backman, H. A., & Dow-Smith, F. (1975). The design and prescription of mutilfocal
lenses for civil pilots. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 52,
591-599.
Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual
attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Investigative
Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 34(11), 3110-3123.
Ball, K., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R. (1990). Developmental changes in attention and
visual search throughout adulthood. In J. T. Enns (Ed.), The development of
attention: research and theory (pp. 489-508). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. L., & Griggs, D. S. (1988). Age and
visual search: expanding the useful field of view. Journal of the Optical Society of
America A-Optics & Image Science, 5(12), 2210-2219.
Beard, B. L., Hisle, W. A., & Ahumada, A. J. (2002). Occupational vision standards: A
review. NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Field: CA: Federal Aviation
Aministration.
Bichao, I. C., Yager, D., & Meng, J. (1995). Disability glare: Effects of temporal
characteristics of the glare source and of the visual-field location of the test stimulus.
Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science & Vision,
12(10), 2252-2258.
Bickel, P. W., & Barr, J. T. (1997). Rigid gas-permeable contact lenses in high and low
humidity. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 68(9), 574-578.
Birch, J., & Dain, S. J. (1999). Performance of red-green color deficient subjects on the
Vision Standards for Aircrew
65
Farnsworth Lantern (FALANT). Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 70(1),
62-67.
Bowman, K. J., & Cole, B. L. (1981). Recognition of the aircraft navigation light color
code. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 52(11 Pt 1), 658-665.
Brabyn, J. A., Haegerstroem-Portnoy, G., Schneck, M. E. & Lott, L. A. (2000). Visual
impairments in elderly people under everyday viewing conditions. Journal of Visual
Impairment & Blindness., 94(12), 741-755.
Braithwaite, M. G., Douglass, P. K., Durnford, S. J., & Lucas, G. (1998). The hazard of
spatial disorientation during helicopter flight using night vision devices. Aviation
Space & Environmental Medicine, 69(11), 1038-1044.
Buckingham, R. S., Cornforth, L. L., Whitwell, K. J., & Lee, R. B. (2003). Visual acuity,
optical, and eye health readiness in the military. Military Medicine, 168(3), 194-198.
Bullimore, M. A., Bailey, I. L., & Wacker, R. T. (1991). Face recognition in age-related
maculopathy. Investigative Ophthalmology and Vision Science, 32, 2020-2029.
Carmean, G. (1998). Establishing Occupational Vision Requirements for Correctional
Officers. CTM Magazine. March.
Carter, T. L. (1994). Age-related vision changes: a primary care guide. Geriatrics, 49(9),
37-42.
Casson, E. J. (1995). Report on the feasibility of developing task-oriented visual
standards for the Canadian Forces (No. DDS #08SV.W8477-4-SC07). Ottawa, ON:
Ottawa University Eye Institute.
Casson, E. J., Gibbs, G. G., & Cameron, B. (1999a). Vision requirements for job tasks of
CF Avionics trade (No. MOC526): BCRI for Department of Medical Services,
Department of National Defence.
Casson, E. J., Gibbs, G. G., & Cameron, B. (1999b). Vision requirements for job tasks of
CF Boatswains (MOC181) (No. MOC181): BCRI for the Department of Medical
Services, Department of National Defence.
Chisholm, C. M., Evans, A. D., Harlow, J. A., & Barbur, J. L. (2003). New test to assess
pilot's vision following refractive surgery. Aviation Space & Environmental
Medicine, 74(5), 551-559.
Christ, R. E. (1975). Review and analysis of color coding research for visual displays.
Human Factors, 17(6), 542-570.
Cole, B. L. (1993). Does defective colour vision really matter? In B. Drum (Ed.), Colour
Vision Deficiencies XI (pp. 67-86). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
66
Cole, B. L. (2004). The handicap of abnormal colour vision. Clinical & Experimental
Optometry, 87(4-5), 258-275.
Cole, B. L., & Macdonald, W. A. (1988). Defective colour vision can impede information
acquisition from redundantly colour-coded video displays.[erratum appears in
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1988;8(3):359]. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 8(2),
198-210.
D'Zmura. (1991). Color in visual search. Vision Research, 31(6), 951-966.
DeVilbiss, C. A., Ercoline, W. R., & Antonio, J. C. (1994). Visual performance with
night vision goggles (NVGs) measured in USAF aircrew members. In R. J.
Lewandowski, W. Stephens & L. A. Haworth (Eds.), SPIE Proceedings, Helmet-
and Helmet-Mounted Displays and Symbology Design Requirements (Vol. 2218, pp.
64-70): SPIE-The International Society for Optical Engineering.
Diepgen, R. (1993). Do pilots need stereopsis? Klinische Monatsblatter fur
Augenheilkunde, 202(2), 94-101.
Donderi, D. C. (1994). Visual acuity, color vision, and visual search performance at sea.
Human Factors, 36(1), 129-144.
Donderi, D. C., Kawaja, K. M., Smiley, A., Henderson, A. S., & Zadra, A. (1994). Vision
and work at sea: The relationships between visual acuity and colour vision and
performance on deck, engineering and logistic tasks of the Candian Coast Guard
(Vol. # TP12139E): Transportation Development Centre, Policy and Coordination,
Transport Canada.
Draeger, J., Brandl, H., Wirt, H., & Burchard, E. (1989). Experimental tests on the
minimal visual acuity required for safe aircrew and air control personnel
performance. AGARD Conference Proceedings Situational Awareness in Aerospace
Operations (No. AGARD-CP-478).
Draeger, J. (1988). Experimental investigations of the visual acuity required fpr the safe
operation of an aircraft. Wehrmedizinische Montaschrift, 1, 6-11.
Elliot, D. B., Whitaker, D., & MacVeigh, D. (1990). Neural contribution to
spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity decline in healthy ageing eyes. Vision Research,
30, 541-547.
Elliott, D. B., & Bullimore, M. A. (1993). Assessing the reliability, discriminative ability,
and validity of disability glare tests. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,
34(1), 108-119.
Elliott, D. B., & Whitaker, D. (1990). Changes in macular function throughout adulthood.
Documenta Ophthalmologica, 76(3), 251-259.
Elliott, D. B., & Whitaker, D. (1992). Clinical contrast sensitivity chart evaluation.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
67
Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 12(3), 275-280.
Elliott, D. B., Yang, K. C., & Whitaker, D. (1995). Visual acuity changes throughout
adulthood in normal, healthy eyes: seeing beyond 6/6. Optometry & Vision Science,
72(3), 186-191.
Erneston, A. G., Ricks, M. R., Tate, T. J., & Ana, R. S. (1996). Vision readiness in the
United States Air Force revisited. Military Medicine, 161(1), 27-28.
Evans, D. W., & Ginsburg, A. P. (1985). Contrast sensitivity predicts age differences in
highway sign discriminability. Human Factors, 23, 59-64.
Eyraud, M. Y., & Borowsky, M. S. (1985). Age and pilot performance. Aviation, Space
and Environmental Medicine, 56(6), 553-558.
Farr, W. D. (1989). Compatibility of the aviation night vision imaging systems and the
aging aviator. Aviation, Space, & Environmental Medicine, 60(10, B78-B80.
Flach, J. M., Warren, R., Garness, S. A., Kelly, L., & Stanard, T. (1997). Perception and
control of altitude: splay and depression angles. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23(6), 1764-1782.
Fowlkes, J. E., Kennedy, R. S., Hettinger, L. J., & Harm, D. L. (1993). Changes in the
dark focus of accommodation associated with simulator sickness. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 64(7), 612-618.
Foyle, D. C., Kaiser, M. K., & Johnson, W. W. (1992). Visual cues in low-level flight:
Implicatoins for pilotage, training, simulation, and enhanced/synthetic vision
systems. American Helicopter Society 48th Annual Forum, 1, 253-260.
Froom, P., Biger, Y., Erel, J., Davidson, B., & Shochat, I. (1992). The incidence of
myopia in the Israel Air Force rated population: a 10-year prospective study.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 63(4), 299-301.
Froom, P., Ribak, J., Burger, A., & Gross, M. (1987). Visual acuity, corrective lenses,
and accidents in helicopter pilots. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 58(3),
252-253.
Ginsburg, A. P., & Easterly, D. W. (1983). Contrast sensitivity predicts target detection
field performance of pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society, 27, 269-273.
Ginsburg, A. P., Evans, D. W., Sekuler, R., & Harp, S. A. (1982). Contrast sensitivity
predicts pilots' performance in aircraft simulators. American Journal of Optometry
& Physiological Optics, 59(1), 105-109.
Gittings, N. S., & Fozard, J. L. (1986). Age changes in visual acuity. Experimental
Gerontology, 21, 423-434.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
68
Glovinsky, Y., Belkin, M., & Hammer, A. (1992). Night vision in young individuals.
Correlation between laboratory findings and performance in the field. Investigative
Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 33(4), 3621.
Good, G. W., & Augsburger, A. R. (1987). Uncorrected visual acuity standards for police
applicants. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15, 18-23.
Good, G. W., Weaver, J. L., & Augsburger, A. R. (1996). Determination and application
of vision standards in industry. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 30(5),
633-640.
Goodman, R. L., Johnson, D. A., Dillon, H., Edelhauser, H. F., & Waller, S. G. (2003).
Laser in situ keratomileusis flap stability during simulated aircraft ejection in a
rabbit model. Cornea, 22(2), 142-145.
Gray, G. W., & McFadden, S. M. (1987). The measurement of contrast censitivity in
aircrew candidates (No. DCIEM No. 87-RR-04). Downsview, Ontario: DCIEM.
Gray, R., & Regan, D. (2000). Simulated self-motion alters perceived time to collision.
Current Biology, 10(10), 587-590.
Grimson, J. M., Schallhorn, S. C., & Kaupp, S. E. (2002). Contrast sensitivity:
establishing normative data for use in screening prospective naval pilots. Aviation
Space & Environmental Medicine, 73(1), 28-35.
Gudmundsdottir, E., Jonasson, F., Jonsson, V., Stefansson, E., Sasaki, H., & Sasaki, K.
(2000). "With the rule" astigmatism is not the rule in the elderly. Reykjavik Eye
Study: a population based study of refraction and visual acuity in citizens of
Reykjavik 50 years and older. Iceland-Japan Co-Working Study Groups. Acta
Ophthalmologica Scandinavica, 78(6), 642-646.
Guirao, A., Gonzalez, C., Redondo, M., Geraghty, E., Norrby, S., & Artal, P. (1999).
Average optical performance of the human eye as a function of age in a normal
population. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 40, 203-213.
Hawkins, B. S. (1995). Reliability of visual acuity measurements and screening under
field conditions. Ophthalmic Epidemiology, 2(2), 99-106.
Hedin, A., & Lovsund, P. (1987). Effects of visual field defects on driving performance.
Documenta Ophthalomologica Proceedings Series, 49, 541-547.
Heikens, M. F., Gray, G. W., O'Neill, H.J., Salisbury, D.A. (1999). 1997 Canadian
Forces Air Operations Vision Survey; Section 1: Operational visual requirements.
Toronto, ON, DCIEM: 36.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
69
Hitchcock, E. M., Dick, R. B., & Krieg, E. F. (2004). Visual contrast sensitivity testing:
A comparison of two F.A.C.T. test types. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 26, 271-
278.
Holmes, S. R., Bunting, A., Brown, D. L., Hiatt, K. L., Braithwaite, M. G., & Harrigan,
M. J. (2003). Survey of spatial disorientation in military pilots and navigators.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 74(9), 957-965.
Holt, J.L. (2002). Arizona verdict for pilot provides lessens on Lasik. (Post v.University
of Arizona). Trial, 38(8), 70-72.
Hong, X., & Regan, D. (1989). Visual field defects for unidirectional and oscillatory
motion in depth. Vision Research, 29(7), 809-819.
Horton, P., & Joseph, C. (2002). Optometrists Association Australia position statement
on driver vision standards. Clinical & Experimental Optometry, 85(4), 241-245.
Hovis, J. K. (2000). Establishing a bona fide occupational requirement and validated
standards for vision: A review. In N. Gledhill, J. Bonneau & A. Salmon (Eds.),
Proceedings of the consensus forum on establishing Bona Fide requirements for
physically demanding occupations (pp. 123-133). Toronto, ON.
Hovis, J. K., & Oliphant, D. (2000). A lantern color vision test for the rail industry.
American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 38(6), 681-696.
Howard, C. M., Riegler, J. T., & Martin, J. J. (2001). Light adaptation: night vision
goggle effect on cockpit instrument reading time. Aviation Space & Environmental
Medicine, 72(6), 529-533.
Hughes, P. C., & Neer, R. M. (1981). Lighting for the elderly: a psychobiological
approach to lighting. Human Factors, 23(1), 65-85.
Hughes, P. K., & Vingrys, A. J. (1991). Reduced contrast sensitivity when viewing
through an aircraft windscreen. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 62(3),
254-257.
Jackson, G. R., & Owsley, C. (2000). Scotopic sensitivity during adulthood. Vision
Research, 40, 2467-2473.
Johnson, C. A., & Casson, E. J. (1995). Effects of luminance, contrast and blur on visual
acuity. Optometry and Vision Science, 72(12), 864-869.
Johnson, C. A., Casson, E. J., & Zadnik, K. (1992). Final report on entry-level
correctional office vision standards for the California Department of Corrections:
California Department of Corrections Technical Report.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
70
Johnson, C. A., & Keltner, J. L. (1983). Incidence of visual field loss in 20,000 eyes and
its relationship to driving performance. Archives of Ophthalmology, 101(3), 371-
375.
Jones, R. K., & Lee, D. (1981). Why two eyes are beter than one: The two views of
binocular vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 7, 30-40.
Kiel, A. W., Butler, T., & Alwitry, A. (2003). Visual acuity and legal visual requirement
to drive a passenger vehicle. Eye, 17(3), 579-582.
Kikukawa, A., Yagura, S., & Akamatsu, T. (1999). A 25-year prospective study of visual
acuity in the Japan Air Self Defense Force personnel. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 70(5), 447-450.
King, V. (1972). Discomfort glare from flashing sources. Journal of the American
Optometric Association, 43(1), 53-56.
Kinnear, P. R., & Sahraie, A. (2002). New Farnsworth-Munsell 100 hue test norms of
normal observers for each year of age 5-22 and for age decades 30-70. British
Journal of Ophthalmology, 86(12), 1408-1411.
Klein, R., Klein, B. E., Lee, K. E., Cruickshanks, K. J., & Chappell, R. J. (2001).
Changes in visual acuity in a population over a 10-year period: The Beaver Dam
Eye Study. Ophthalmology, 108(10), 1757-1766.
Kline, D., Caird, J. K., Ho, G., & Dewar, R. E. (2002). Analytic study to assess the visual
deficits of aging drivers and the legibility of on-board intelligent transport system
(its) displays. Final report. Ottawa, ON: Transport Canada.
Kline, D. W., Culham, J. C., Bartel, P., & Lynk, L. (2001). Aging effects on vernier
acuity: A function of oscillation rate but not target contrast. Optometry and Vision
Science, 78, 676-682.
Kline, D. W., Kline, T. J. B., Fozard, J. L., Kosnik, W., Schieber, F., & Sekuler, R.
(1992). Vision, aging and driving: The problems of older drivers. Journal of
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47, 27-34.
Kline, D. W., Schieber, F., Abusamra, L. A., & Coyne, A. (1983). Age, the eye, and the
visual channels: Contrast sensitivity and response speed. Journal of Gerontology,
38, 211-216.
Kline, D. W., & Scialfa, C. T. (1996). Visual and auditory aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W.
Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the Psychology of Aging (4th ed., pp. 181-203). San
Diego: Academic Press.
Kochhar, D. S., & Fraser, T. M. (1978). Monocular peripheral vision as a factor in flight
safety. Aviation, Space, & Environmental Medicine, 49, 698-706.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
71
Kotulak, J. C., & Morse, S. E. (1994). Focus adjustment effects on visual acuity and
oculomotor balance with aviator night vision displays. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 65(4), 348-352.
Kruk, R., & Regan, D. (1996). Collision avoidance: a helicopter simulator study. Aviation
Space & Environmental Medicine, 67(2), 111-114.
Levy, Y., & Glovinsky, Y. (1997). Evaluation of mid-term stability of night vision tests.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 68(7), 565-568.
Levy, Y., Zadok, D., & Barenboim, E. (2003). Laser in situ keratomileusis in a combat jet
aircraft pilot. Journal of Cataract & Refractive Surgery, 29(6), 1239-1241.
Lintern, G., & Liu, Y. T. (1991). Explicit and implicit horizons for simulated landing
approaches. Human Factors, 33(4), 401-417.
Luder, C. B., & Barber, P. J. (1984). Redundant color coding on airborne CRT displays.
Human Factors, 26(1), 19-32.
Macdonald, W. A., & Cole, B.L. (1988). Evaluating the role of colour in flight
information. Ergonomics, 31(1), 13-37.
Mann, J., & Hovis, J. (1996). The effect of optical blur on simulated IFR approaches.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 67(8), 739-745.
Manton, A. G. (2000). Night vision goggles, human factors aspects--a questionnaire
survey of helicopter aircrew. Journal of the Royal Army Medical Corps, 146(1), 22-
27.
Markovits, A. S. (1993). Photo-refractive keratectomy (PRK): threat or millennium for
military pilots? Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 64(5), 409-411.
Markovits, W. F., Reddix, M. D., O'Connell, S. R., & Collyer, P. D. (1995). Comparison
of bifocal and progresive addition lenses on aviator taget detection performance.
Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 66, 303-308.
McFadden, S. M. (1982). Analysis of the possible effects on vision of exposure to a
powerful flash lamp (No. TC-82-C-14). Downsview, ON: Defence and Civil
Institute of Environmental Medicine.
McFadden, S. M. (1994). A comparison of two contrast sensitivity tests and their
usefulness as a screener for aircrew. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine,
65(8), 710-717.
McFadden, S. M., & Kaufmann, R. (1993). Canadian forces evaluation of contrast
sentivitiy for measuring visual capability. Downsview, ON: Defence and Civil
Institute of Environmental Medicine.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
72
Mertens, H. W. (1978). Comparison of the visual perception of a runway model in pilots
and nonpilots during simulated night landing approaches. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 49(9), 1043-1055.
Mertens, H. W., & Milburn, N. J. (1998). Validity of clinical color vision tests for air
traffic control. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 69(7), 666-674.
Miller, R. E., 2nd, Woessner, W. M., Dennis, R. J., O'Neal, M. R., & Green, R. P., Jr.
(1990). Survey of spectacle wear and refractive error prevalence in USAF pilots and
navigators. Optometry & Vision Science, 67(11), 833-839.
Mittelman, M. H., Siegel, B., & Still, D. L. (1993). Contact lenses in aviation: the Marine
Corps experience. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 64(6), 538-540.
Miura, Y., Shoji, M., Fukumoto, M., Yasue, K., Tsukui, I., & Hosoya, T. (2002). A 10-
year retrospective review of airline transport pilots aged 60 to 63 in Japan. Aviation
Space & Environmental Medicine, 73(5), 485-487.
Mork, M. R., & Watson, L. A. (1993). Prevalence of corrective lens wear in Royal
Australian Air Force flight crews. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 64(6),
541-545.
Myers, R. S., Ball, K. K., Kalina, T. D., Roth, D. L., & Goode, K. T. (2000). Relation of
the useful field of view and other screening tests to on-road driving. Perceptual and
Psychomotor Skills, 91(1), 279-290.
Nakagawara, V. B., Montgomery, R. W. & Wood, K. J. (2002). "Aviation accidents and
incidents associated with the use of ophthalmic devices by civilian airmen."
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine 73(11): 1109-13.
Nakagawara, V. B., Montgomery, R. W., & Wood, K. J. (2004). Changing demographics
and vision restrictions in civilian pilots and their clinical implications. Aviation
Space & Environmental Medicine, 75(9), 785-790.
Nakagawara, V. B., Wood, K. J., & Montgomery, R. W. (1995). Vision impairment and
corrective considerations of civil airmen.[erratum appears in J Am Optom Assoc
1995 Oct;66(10):602]. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 66(8), 489-
494.
Nakagawara, V. B., Wood, K. J., & Montgomery, R. W. (2002). The use of contact
lenses by U.S. civilian pilots. Optometry (St Louis, Mo), 73(11), 674-684.
Nakagawara, V. B., Wood, K. J., & Montgomery, R. W. (2004). Natural sunlight and its
association to civil aviation accidents. Optometry (St Louis, Mo), 75(8), 517-522.
National Research Council Committee on Vision. (1985). Emergent techniques for
assessment of visual peformance, Techical report: National Academy of Sciences
Press.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
73
Nikolic, M. I., & Sarter, N. B. (2001). Peripheral visual feedback: a powerful means of
supporting effective attention allocation in event-driven, data-rich environments.
Human Factors, 43(1), 30-38.
O'Brien, K. A., Cole, B. L., Maddocks, J. D., & Forbes, A. B. (2002). Color and defective
color vision as factors in the conspicuity of signs and signals. Human Factors, 44(4),
665-675.
O'Connell, S. R., & Markovits, A. S. (1995). The fate of eyewear in aircraft ejections.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 66(2), 104-107.
Otakeno, S., Matthews, R. S., Folio, L., Previc, F. H., & Lessard, C. S. (2002). The
effects of visual scenes on roll and pitch thresholds in pilots versus nonpilots.
Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 73(2), 98-101.
Owsley, C., Sekuler, R., & Siemsen, D. (1983). Contrast sensitivity throughout
adulthood. Vision Research, 23(7), 689-699.
Owsley, C., & Sloane, M. E. (1987). Contrast sensitivity, acuity and the perception of
real-world targets. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 71, 791-796.
Pelli, D. G., Robson, J.G., & Wilkins, A.J. (1988). The design of a new letter chart for
measuring contrast sensitivity. Clinical Vision Science, 2, 187-199.
Pesudovs, K., Hazel, C. A., Doran, R. M. L., & Elliott, D. B. (2004). The usefulness of
Vistech and FACT contrast sensitivity charts for cataract and refractive surgery
outcomes research. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 88, 11-16.
Polishuk, A., & Raz, D. (1975). Soft hydrophilic contact lenses in civil and military
aviation. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 46(9), 1188-1190.
Prinzel, L. J. (2004). Head-up displays and attention capture (No. NASA/TM-2004-
213000). Washington, D.C.: National Aeronautics and Space Administration.
Provines, W. F., Woessner, W. M., Rahe, A. J., & Tredici, T. J. (1983). The incidence of
refractive anomalies in the USAF rated population. Aviation Space & Environmental
Medicine, 54(7), 622-627.
Rabin, J. (1993). Spatial contrast sensitivity through aviator's night vision imaging
system. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 64(8), 706-710.
Rabin, J. C. (1994). Optical defocus: Differential effects on size and contrast letter
recognition thresholds. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 35, 646-648.
Rabin, J. C. (1995). Luminance effects on visual acuity and small letter contrast
sensitivity.
Rabin, J. C. (1996). Measuring resolution in the contrast domain: the small letter
Vision Standards for Aircrew
74
contrast test (Reprint) [bnhv] (Final report ed.). US Army Aeromedical Resea: Fort
Rucker, AL; USAARL.
Regan, D. (1995). Spatial orientation in aviation: visual contributions. Journal of
Vestibular Research, 5(6), 455-471.
Regan, D., & Gray, R. (2001). Hitting what one wants to hit and missing what one wants
to miss. Vision Research, 41(25-26), 3321-3329.
Regan, D., & Neima, D. (1983). Low-contrast letter charts as a test of visual function.
Ophthalmology, 90(10), 1192-1200.
Rogé, J., Pebayle, T., Hannachi, S. E., & Muzet, A. (2003). Effect of sleep deprivation
and driving duration on the useful visual field in younger and older subjects during
simulator driving. Vision Research, 43(13), 1465-1472.
Roy, G. S., & Choudhary, R. K. (1985). Driver control as a factor in road safety. Asian
Journal of Psychology & Education, 16(3), 33-37.
Saur, R. L. (1969). Influence of physiological factors on discomfort glare level. American
Journal of Optometry & Archives of American Academy of Optometry, 46(5), 352-
357.
Schallhorn, S. C., Blanton, C. L., Kaupp, S. E., Sutphin, J., Gordon, M., Goforth, H., Jr.,
& Butler, F.K, Jr. (1996). Preliminary results of photorefractive keratectomy in
active-duty United States Navy personnel. Ophthalmology, 103(1), 5-22.
Schieber, F. (1994). Age and glare recovery time for low-contrast stimuli. Proceedings of
the 38th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors Society, 38(1994), 496-500.
Schieber, F., Kline, D. W., Kline, T. J. B., & Fozard, J. L. (1992). The relationship
between contrast sensitivity and the visual problems of older drivers, Society of
Automotive Engineering Technical Paper (No. 920613). Warrendale, PA: Society of
Automotive Engineers.
Schlote, T., Derse, M., Wannke, B., Bende, T., & Jean, B. (1999). Impairment of mesopic
vision following photorefractive keratectomy of myopia. Klinische Monatsblatter
fur Augenheilkunde, 214(3), 136-141.
Schneck, M. E., Haegerstrom-Portnoy, G., Lott, L. A., Brabyn, J. A., & Gildengorin, G.
(2004). Low contrast vision function predicts subsequent acuity loss in an aged
population: the SKI study. Vision Research, 44, 2317-2325.
Scholz, R., Andresen, S., Hofmann, H., & Duncker, G. (1995). Recognition performance
of subjects with color-vision deficiencies on a polychromatic sonar screen for ship
navigation. German Journal of Ophthalmology, 4(2), 103-106.
Schwartz, R., Stern, C., Klemm, M., Draeger, J., & Winter, R. (1996). Glaucoma and
Vision Standards for Aircrew
75
aircraft pilot fitness. Ophthalmologe, 93(1), 76-79.
Scialfa, C. T., Guzy, L. T., Leibowitz, H. W., Garvey, P. M., & Tyrrell, R. A. (1991).
Age differences in estimating vehicle velocity. Psychology and Aging, 6, 60-66.
Scialfa, C. T., Kline, D. W., & Lyman, B. J. (1987). Age differences in target
identification as a function of retinal location and noise level: Examination of the
useful field of view. Psychology and Aging, 2, 14-19.
Sekuler, A. B., Bennett, P. J., & Mamelak, M. (2000). Effects of aging on the useful field
of view. Experimental Aging Research, 26(2), 103-120.
Sekuler, R., Kline, D., & Dismukes, K. (1982). Aging and visual function of military
pilots: a review. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 53(8), 747-758.
Sheedy, J. E. (1980). Police vision standards. Journal of Police Science and
Administration, 8, 275-285.
Sheehy, J. B., & Wilkinson, M. (1989). Depth perception after prolonged usage of night
vision goggles. Aviation, Space, & Environmental Medicine, 60(6), 573-579.
Silberman, W. S., Apsey, D., Ivan, D. J., Jackson, W. G., & Mitchell, G. W. (1994). The
effect of test chart design and human factors on visual performance with night vision
goggles. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 65(12), 1077-1081.
Snyder, Q. C., Jr., & Lezotte, D. C. (1993). Prospective assessment of stereoscopic visual
status and USAF pilot training attrition. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine,
64(1), 14-19.
Spierer, A., & Shalev, B. (2003). Presbyopia among normal individuals. Graefes Archive
for Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, 241(2), 101-105.
Stager, P., & Hameluck, D. (1986). Contrast sensitivity and visual detection in search
and rescue. Final Report: DCIEM.
Stern, C. (1999). New refractive surgery procedures in ophthalmology and the influence
on Pilot's fitness for flying. European Journal of Medical Research, 4(9), 382-384.
Steward, S. M., & Cole, B. L. (1989). What do colour vision defectives say about
everyday tasks? Optometry and Vision Science, 66, 288-295.
Stewart, J. D., & Clark, B. (1975). Choice reaction time to visual motion during
prolonged rotary motion in airline pilots. Aviation Space & Environmental
Medicine, 46(6), 767-771.
Still, D. L., & Temme, L. A. (1992). Eyeglass use by U.S. Navy jet pilots: effects on
night carrier landing performance. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine,
63(4), 273-275.
Vision Standards for Aircrew
76
Stone, L. W., Sanders, M. G., Glick, D. D., Wiley, R. W., & Kimball, K. A. (1980). A
human performance/workload evaluation of the AN/PVS-5 bifocal night vision
goggles. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 51(8), 797-804.
Swamy, S., Joseph, C., Aravind, A. S., & Vevai, R. J. (2002). Contrast sensitivity in IAF
aircrew. Indian Journal of Aerospace Medicine, 46(2), 7-22.
Szafran, J. (1969). Psychological studies of aging in pilots. Aerospace Medicine, 40(5),
543-553.
Szlyk, J. P., Brigell, M., & Seiple, W. (1993). Effects of age and hemianopic visual field
loss on driving. Optometry and Vision Science, 70, 1031-1037.
Tan, J. C., Spalton, D. J., & Arden, G. B. (1998). Comparison of methods to assess visual
impairment from glare and light scattering with posterior capsule opacification.
Journal of Cataract & Refractive Surgery, 24(12), 1626-1631.
Tanzer, D. J., Schallhorn, S. C., & Brown, M. C. (2000). Ejection from an aircraft
following photorefractive keratectomy: a case report. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 71(10), 1057-1059.
Task, H. L., & Griffin, L. L. (1982). PAVE LOW III: interior lighting reconfiguration for
night lighting and night vision goggle compatibility. Aviation Space &
Environmental Medicine, 53(12), 1162-1165.
Temme, L. A., & Still, D. L. (1991). Prescriptive eyeglass use by U.S. Navy jet pilots:
effects on air-to-air target detection. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine,
62(9 Pt 1), 823-826.
Temme, L. A., Still, D. L., & Fatcheric, A. J. (1995). Jet pilot, helicopter pilot, and
college student: a comparison of central vision. Aviation Space & Environmental
Medicine, 66(4), 297-302.
Theeuwes, J., Alferdinck, J. W. A. M., & Perel, M. (2002). Relation between glare and
driving performance. Human Factors, 44(1), 95-107.
Trick, G. E., & Silverman, S. E. (1991). Visual sensitivity to motion: Age-related
changes and deficits in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neurology, 41,
1437-1440.
Ungs, T. J. (1989). The occurrence of the vection illusion among helicopter pilots while
flying over water. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 60(11), 1099-1101.
Verdon, W., Maloney, R., & Bullimore, M. (1995). Visual performance following
photorefractive keratectomy. Vision Science and its Applications Technical Digest
Series, 1, 62-65.
Vingrys, A. J., & Cole, B. L. (1986). Origins of colour vision standards within the
Vision Standards for Aircrew
77
transport industry. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 6(4), 369-375.
Vingrys, A. J. C., B.L. (1988). Are colour vision standards justified for the transport
industry? Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 8, 257-274.
Vistech Consultants. (1988). Vistech Contrast Sensitivity Test. Ohio: Vistech Consultants.
Wang, Q., Klein, B. E., Klein, R., & Moss, S. E. (1994). Refractive status in the Beaver
Dam Eye Study. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 35(13), 4344-4347.
Whittaker, S. G., & Lovie-Kitchen, J. (1993). Visual requirements for reading. Optometry
and Vision Science, 70, 54-65.
Wright, C. E., & Wormald, R. P. (1992). Stereopsis and ageing. Eye, 6, 473-476.
Young, K. R., Mershon, D. H., & Hicks, L. J. (2002). The far-anchor effect: errors in the
perception of motion and implications for aviation safety. Human Factors, 44(1),
133-143.
UNCLASSIFIED
DOCUMENT CONTROL DATA
(Security classification of the title, body of abstract and indexing annotation must be entered when the overall document is classified)
1. ORIGINATOR (The name and address of the organization preparing the document, Organizations
for whom the document was prepared, e.g. Centre sponsoring a contractor's report, or tasking agency,
are entered in section 8.)
Publishing: DRDC Toronto
Performing: Greenley &Associates, 5 Corvus Court, Ottawa ON, K2E
7Z4
Monitoring:
Contracting: DRDC Toronto
2. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION
(Overall security classification of the document
including special warning terms if applicable.)
UNCLASSIFIED
3. TITLE (The complete document title as indicated on the title page. Its classification is indicated by the appropriate abbreviation (S, C, R, or U) in parenthesis at
the end of the title)
Vision Standards for Aircrew: Visual Acuity for Pilots (U)
Normes visuelles pour le personnel navigant : acuité visuelle des pilotes
4. AUTHORS (First name, middle initial and last name. If military, show rank, e.g. Maj. John E. Doe.)
Kumagai, Jason K.; Williams, Sheri; Kline, Donald
5. DATE OF PUBLICATION
(Month and year of publication of document.)
March 2005
6a NO. OF PAGES
(Total containing information, including
Annexes, Appendices, etc.)
91
6b. NO. OF REFS
(Total cited in document.)
7. DESCRIPTIVE NOTES (The category of the document, e.g. technical report, technical note or memorandum. If appropriate, enter the type of report, e.g.
interim, progress, summary, annual or final. Give the inclusive dates when a specific reporting period is covered.)
Contract Report
8. SPONSORING ACTIVITY (The names of the department project office or laboratory sponsoring the research and development − include address.)
Sponsoring: D Air PPD 2
Tasking: D Air PPD 2
9a. PROJECT OR GRANT NO. (If appropriate, the applicable
research and development project or grant under which the document was
written. Please specify whether project or grant.)
W7711−047921/001/TOR
9b. CONTRACT NO. (If appropriate, the applicable number under which
the document was written.)
10a. ORIGINATOR'S DOCUMENT NUMBER (The official
document number by which the document is identified by the originating
activity. This number must be unique to this document)
DRDC Toronto CR 2005−142
10b. OTHER DOCUMENT NO(s). (Any other numbers under which
may be assigned this document either by the originator or by the
sponsor.)
11. DOCUMENT AVAILABILIY (Any limitations on the dissemination of the document, other than those imposed by security classification.)
Unlimited distribution
12. DOCUMENT ANNOUNCEMENT (Any limitation to the bibliographic announcement of this document. This will normally correspond to the Document
Availability (11), However, when further distribution (beyond the audience specified in (11) is possible, a wider announcement audience may be selected.))
Unlimited announcement
UNCLASSIFIED
UNCLASSIFIED
DOCUMENT CONTROL DATA
(Security classification of the title, body of abstract and indexing annotation must be entered when the overall document is classified)
13. ABSTRACT (A brief and factual summary of the document. It may also appear elsewhere in the body of the document itself. It is highly desirable that the abstract of
classified documents be unclassified. Each paragraph of the abstract shall begin with an indication of the security classification of the information in the paragraph
(unless the document itself is unclassified) represented as (S), (C), (R), or (U). It is not necessary to include here abstracts in both official languages unless the text is
bilingual.)
(U) This report documents a study investigating the Canadian Forces (CF) aircrew entrance vision standard. A
literature review was conducted to identify a method for establishing bona fide occupational requirements and
validated standards for aircrew related visual functions. A protocol for establishing and validating an
occupationally based visual acuity standard for the CF pilot occupation was selected. Tasks that have critical
visual acuity functions were identified based on data obtained through questionnaires and a focus group
session. The study proposes potential task simulations that accurately reflect critical aircrew tasks and an
experimental plan to establish vision standards.
(U) Ce rapport présente une étude portant sur la norme visuelle fixée pour le personnel navigant au niveau
d’entrée en fonction des Forces canadiennes. Une revue de la littérature visant à trouver une méthode pour
établir des exigences professionnelles justifiées et des normes validées pour les fonctions visuelles du
personnel navigant a été menée. Un protocole d’établissement et de validation d’une norme d’acuité visuelle
pour la profession de pilote des FC a été choisi. Les tâches comportant des fonctions essentielles liées à
l’acuité visuelle ont été définies à partir de données tirées de questionnaires et d’une séance de discussion en
groupe. L’étude propose des simulations de tâches qui reflètent avec exactitude les tâches essentielles du
personnel navigant et un plan expérimental de fixation des normes visuelles.
14. KEYWORDS, DESCRIPTORS or IDENTIFIERS (Technically meaningful terms or short phrases that characterize a document and could be helpful in cataloguing the
document. They should be selected so that no security classification is required. Identifiers, such as equipment model designation, trade name, military project code
name, geographic location may also be included. If possible keywords should be selected from a published thesaurus, e.g. Thesaurus of Engineering and Scientific
Terms (TEST) and that thesaurus identified. If it is not possible to select indexing terms which are Unclassified, the classification of each should be indicated as with
the title.)
(U) Canadian Forces; Aircrew; Vision; Acuity; Contrast Sensitivity; Visual Fields; Glare Sensitivity; Colour Vision;
Night Vision; Depth Perception; Motion Perception; Refractive Error; Air Command; Vision Standard; Pilot
UNCLASSIFIED